Practice-Related Optimization of Dual-Task Performance: Efficient Task Instantiation During Overlapping Task Processing
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Torsten Schubert,et al. The central attentional limitation and executive control. , 2008, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.
[2] T. Schubert,et al. Video game experience and optimized executive control skills—On false positives and false negatives: Reply to Boot and Simons (2012) , 2012 .
[3] L. Hasher,et al. Automatic and effortful processes in memory. , 1979 .
[4] Torsten Schubert,et al. Practice-related optimization and transfer of executive functions: a general review and a specific realization of their mechanisms in dual tasks , 2014, Psychological Research.
[5] Harold Pashler,et al. Effects of practice on task architecture: Combined evidence from interference experiments and random-walk models of decision making , 2011, Cognition.
[6] John F. Larish,et al. Training for attentional control in dual task settings: A comparison of young and old adults , 1995 .
[7] J. C. Johnston,et al. Why practice reduces dual-task interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] Arthur F Kramer,et al. Testing the limits of cognitive plasticity in older adults: application to attentional control. , 2006, Acta psychologica.
[9] R. Shiffrin,et al. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .
[10] Torsten Schubert,et al. Testing the Limits of Optimizing Dual-Task Performance in Younger and Older Adults , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[11] H. Pashler,et al. The specificity of learned parallelism in dual-memory retrieval , 2014, Memory & cognition.
[12] D. Navon,et al. Queuing or Sharing? A Critical Evaluation of the Single-Bottleneck Notion , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.
[13] R avid Ell. Working memory involvement in dual-task performance: Evidence from the backward compatibility effect , 2008 .
[14] N. Lavie. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[15] E. Spelke,et al. Dividing Attention Without Alternation or Automaticity , 1980 .
[16] Richard P. DeShon,et al. Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. , 2002, Psychological methods.
[17] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[18] François Maquestiaux,et al. Can practice overcome age-related differences in the psychological refractory period effect? , 2004, Psychology and aging.
[19] Torsten Schubert,et al. Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks , 1999 .
[20] I. Koch,et al. Crossmodal action selection: Evidence from dual-task compatibility , 2010, Memory & cognition.
[21] J. C. Johnston,et al. Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .
[22] J. C. Johnston,et al. How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? , 2006, Psychological research.
[23] S. Hochstein,et al. Attentional Demands Following Perceptual Skill Training , 2001, Psychological science.
[24] Jennifer M. Glass,et al. Virtually Perfect Time Sharing in Dual-Task Performance: Uncorking the Central Cognitive Bottleneck , 2001, Psychological science.
[25] E. Ruthruff,et al. Learning to bypass the central bottleneck: declining automaticity with advancing age. , 2010, Psychology and aging.
[26] Rolf Ulrich,et al. On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.
[27] Timothy C Rickard,et al. Practice effects on two memory retrievals from a single cue. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[28] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[29] Torsten Schubert,et al. Investigation on the improvement and transfer of dual-task coordination skills , 2012, Psychological research.
[30] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Simultaneous cognitive operations in working memory after dual-task practice. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] A. Kramer,et al. Transfer Effects in Task-Set Cost and Dual-Task Cost After Dual-Task Training in Older and Younger Adults: Further Evidence for Cognitive Plasticity in Attentional Control in Late Adulthood , 2008, Experimental aging research.
[32] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .
[33] R. D. Gordon,et al. Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.
[34] Ritske de Jong,et al. THE ROLE OF PREPARATION IN OVERLAPPING-TASK PERFORMANCE , 1995 .
[35] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The identification and transfer of timesharing skills , 1980 .
[36] H. Müller,et al. Evidence for the acquisition of dual-task coordination skills in older adults. , 2015, Acta psychologica.
[37] S. Belleville,et al. Identifying training modalities to improve multitasking in older adults , 2014, AGE.
[38] E Ruthruff,et al. Can practice eliminate the psychological refractory period effect? , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[39] Scott Watter,et al. PRP training shows Task1 response selection is the locus of the backward response compatibility effect , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[40] J. C. Johnston,et al. Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] David E. Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .
[42] Hillary D. Schwarb,et al. Parallel response selection disrupts sequence learning under dual-task conditions. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[43] F. Tong,et al. Training Improves Multitasking Performance by Increasing the Speed of Information Processing in Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, Neuron.
[44] Torsten Schubert,et al. Effects of extensive dual-task practice on processing stages in simultaneous choice tasks , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[45] H. Müller,et al. Age- and practice-related influences on dual-task costs and compensation mechanisms under optimal conditions of dual-task performance , 2012, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.
[46] G. Band,et al. Reconfiguration and the bottleneck: Does task switching affect the refractory period effect? , 2006 .
[47] Robert Borger,et al. The Refractory Period and Serial Choice-reactions , 1963 .
[48] Torsten Schubert,et al. Modulation of executive control in dual tasks with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) , 2015, Neuropsychologia.
[49] Christina B Reimer,et al. The impact of free-order and sequential-order instructions on task-order regulation in dual tasks , 2018, Psychological research.
[50] R. Kliegl,et al. Age differences in dual-task performance after practice. , 2007, Psychology and aging.
[51] D. V. von Cramon,et al. Localization of Executive Functions in Dual-Task Performance with fMRI , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[52] Nelson Cowan,et al. Metatheory of storage capacity limits , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[53] H. Pashler,et al. Procedural learning. I, Locus of practice effects in speeded choice tasks , 1991 .
[54] A. D. Fisk,et al. Memory as a function of attention, level of processing, and automatization. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[55] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. Virtually no evidence for virtually perfect time-sharing. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] Eliot Hazeltine,et al. Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] Roy Luria,et al. Increased Control Demand Results in Serial Processing , 2005, Psychological science.
[58] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[59] Andrew R. A. Conway,et al. On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.
[60] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[61] J. Townsend,et al. A theory of interactive parallel processing: new capacity measures and predictions for a response time inequality series. , 2004, Psychological review.
[62] T. Schubert,et al. Does Initial Performance Variability Predict Dual-Task Optimization with Practice in Younger and Older Adults? , 2015, Experimental aging research.
[63] R. Proctor,et al. Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[64] Torsten Schubert,et al. Transferability of Dual-Task Coordination Skills after Practice with Changing Component Tasks , 2017, Front. Psychol..
[65] G. A. Miller. THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .
[66] Scott Watter. Parallel Response Selection in Dual -Tasks , 2003 .
[67] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Bimanual Response Grouping in Dual-Task Paradigms , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[68] Ritske De Jong,et al. The Role of Preparation in Overlapping-task Performance , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[69] Torsten Schubert,et al. The effect of task order predictability in audio-visual dual task performance: Just a central capacity limitation? , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[70] T. Schubert,et al. No Evidence for Task Automatization After Dual-Task Training in Younger and Older Adults , 2017, Psychology and aging.
[71] Torsten Schubert,et al. Improved Intertask Coordination after Extensive Dual-Task Practice , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[72] Camarin E. Rolle,et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults , 2013, Nature.
[73] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[74] N. Lavie. Attention, Distraction, and Cognitive Control Under Load , 2010 .
[75] Torsten Schubert,et al. Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex: an event-related fMRI study , 2006, Psychological research.
[76] N. Kanwisher,et al. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Provides New Constraints on Theories of the Psychological Refractory Period , 2004, Psychological science.
[77] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Response grouping in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm: Models and contamination effects , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.
[78] Eric Ruthruff,et al. Bypassing the central bottleneck after single-task practice in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Evidence for task automatization and greedy resource recruitment , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[79] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Serial and parallel processes in working memory after practice. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[80] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[81] A. Hartley,et al. Age-related differences and similarities in dual-task interference. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[82] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Design for a working memory. , 2009 .
[83] Jacob Cohen,et al. A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.