Two-year-olds' search strategies and visual tracking in a hidden displacement task.

Children younger than 3 years have difficulty with search tasks that involve hidden displacement. Partial visual information was provided about a ball's path as it moved toward a hiding place. Children (2.0 and 2.5 years old) saw a ball rolling down a ramp placed behind a transparent screen with 4 opaque doors. A wall, placed on the ramp and directly behind 1 of the doors, protruded above the screen and stopped the ball. Children were asked to find the ball. The transparency of the screen permitted visual tracking of the ball between the doors, but its final resting place was obscured. Both age groups were equally proficient at tracking the ball as it rolled behind the screen, but the 2.5-year-olds were more likely to reach to the correct door. Looking behavior was related to errors in the younger group in that tracking that stopped short or continued past the correct door was associated with incorrect choices.

[1]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[2]  M. W. Daehler,et al.  The Effects of Distraction and Added Perceptual Cues on the Delayed Reaction of Very Young Children. , 1973 .

[3]  N. A. Myers,et al.  The effects of size and color cues on the delayed response of very young children , 1976 .

[4]  J. G. Bremner,et al.  Place versus response as the basis of spatial errors made by young infants. , 1977, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  L. Acredolo Development of Spatial Orientation in Infancy. , 1978 .

[6]  J. G. Bremner,et al.  Spatial errors made by infants: inadequate spatial cues or evidence of egocentrism? , 1978, British journal of psychology.

[7]  C Sophian,et al.  Developments in infants' search for displaced objects. , 1983, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[8]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Children's Searching : The Development of Search Skill and Spatial Representation , 1985 .

[9]  J. Deloache Memory in very young children: Exploitation of cues to the location of a hidden object , 1986 .

[10]  Leslie B. Cohen,et al.  From perception to cognition , 1987 .

[11]  W. P. Dixon,et al.  BMPD statistical software manual , 1988 .

[12]  Renée Baillargeon,et al.  Why do young infants fail to search for hidden objects? , 1990, Cognition.

[13]  E. Spelke,et al.  Origins of knowledge. , 1992, Psychological review.

[14]  R. Bogartz An introduction to the analysis of variance , 1994 .

[15]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  The Coding of Spatial Location in Young Children , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  Carl E. Granrud,et al.  Visual perception and cognition in infancy , 1995 .

[17]  B. Hood Gravity rules for 2- to 4-year olds? , 1995 .

[18]  J. Reznick,et al.  Response modality affects human infant delayed-response performance. , 1996, Child development.

[19]  J. G. Bremner,et al.  Infant Development: Recent Advances , 1997 .

[20]  Adele Diamond,et al.  Understanding the A‐not‐B Error: Working memory vs. reinforced response, or active trace vs. latent trace , 1998 .

[21]  E. Spelke Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge , 1998 .

[22]  B. Hood Gravity does rule for falling events , 1998 .

[23]  L. Mcdonough Early declarative memory for location , 1999 .

[24]  Ellin Kofsky Scholnick,et al.  Conceptual Development : Piaget's Legacy , 1999 .

[25]  S. Carey,et al.  Predicting the outcomes of physical events: two-year-olds fail to reveal knowledge of solidity and support. , 2000, Child development.

[26]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Making Space: The Development of Spatial Representation and Reasoning , 2000 .

[27]  R. K. Clifton Lessons From Infants: 1960-2000. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.