Simulating believable forward accelerations on a stewart motion platform

It is still an unsolved problem how to optimally simulate self-motion using motion simulators. We investigated how a forward acceleration can be simulated as believably as possible on a hexapod motion platform equipped with a projection screen. Human participants rated the believability of brief forward accelerations. These were simulated as visual forward accelerations over a ground plane with people as size cues, presented together with brief forward surge translations and backward pitches of the platform, and synchronous random up--down movements of the camera in the visual scene and the platform. The magnitudes of all of the parameters were varied independently across trials. Even though variability between participants was high, the most believable simulation occurred when visual accelerations were combined with backward pitches of the platform, which changed the gravitoinertial vector direction approximately consistent with the visual acceleration. However, a wide range of platform pitches was accepted as believable. With high visual acceleration cues most participants reported trials as realistic even when the platform tilt rate was above vestibular canal thresholds reported in other works. Other manipulated parameters had only a mild influence on the responses. These results can be used to optimize motion-cueing algorithms for simulating linear accelerations in motion simulators.

[1]  Ruud Hosman,et al.  PSYCHOPHYSICAL THRESHOLDS ASSOCIATED WITH THE SIMULATION OF LINEAR ACCELERATION , 2000 .

[2]  Meyer Nahon,et al.  Response of airline pilots to variations in flight simulator motion algorithms , 1988 .

[3]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Limited Field of View of Head-Mounted Displays Is Not the Cause of Distance Underestimation in Virtual Environments , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[4]  H Kingma,et al.  Thresholds for perception of direction of linear acceleration as a possible evaluation of the otolith function , 2005, BMC ear, nose, and throat disorders.

[5]  Laurence R. Harris,et al.  Travel distance estimation from visual motion by leaky path integration , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Multisensory integration in the estimation of relative path length , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Effects of Stereo Viewing Conditions on Distance Perception in Virtual Environments , 2008, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[8]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  The Contributions of Static Visual Cues, Nonvisual Cues, and Optic Flow in Distance Estimation , 2004, Perception.

[9]  D M Merfeld,et al.  Humans use internal models to estimate gravity and linear acceleration , 1999, Nature.

[10]  M. Gresty,et al.  Thresholds for detection of motion direction during passive lateral whole-body acceleration in normal subjects and patients with bilateral loss of labyrinthine function , 1996, Brain Research Bulletin.

[11]  A. J. Benson,et al.  Thresholds for the perception of whole body angular movement about a vertical axis. , 1989, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[12]  S Palmisano,et al.  Global-Perspective Jitter Improves Vection in Central Vision , 2000, Perception.

[13]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. , 2003 .

[14]  M. Alexander,et al.  Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .

[15]  Frank M. Cardullo,et al.  Motion Cueing Algorithm Development: Human-Centered Linear and Nonlinear Approaches , 2005 .

[16]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Linear Self Motion Perception , 1982 .

[17]  W Bles,et al.  How to use body tilt for the simulation of linear self motion. , 2004, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[18]  Ludovic Marin,et al.  Contribution of Somesthetic Information to the Perception of Body Orientation in the Pitch Dimension , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[19]  A. J. Benson,et al.  Thresholds for the detection of the direction of whole-body, linear movement in the horizontal plane. , 1986, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[20]  A Berthoz,et al.  Spatial memory of body linear displacement: what is being stored? , 1995, Science.

[21]  D. N. Lee The optic flow field: the foundation of vision. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[22]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  A Bayesian model of the disambiguation of gravitoinertial force by visual cues , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  L. Young,et al.  Vestibular nucleus units in alert monkeys are also influenced by moving visual fields. , 1974, Brain research.

[24]  L R Young,et al.  Interaction of optokinetic and vestibular stimuli in motion perception. , 1973, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[25]  P M Baudonnière,et al.  Vestibular Sensitivity and Vection Chronometry along the Spinal Axis in Erect Man , 1999, Perception.

[26]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection) basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions , 1975, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Merging the senses into a robust percept , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  B. J. Yates,et al.  Responses of vestibular nucleus neurons to tilt following chronic bilateral removal of vestibular inputs , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Vincent Nougier,et al.  Perceived body orientation in microgravity: effects of prior experience and pressure under the feet. , 2004, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[30]  Ruud Hosman,et al.  Evaluation of Perceived Motion During a Simulated Takeoff Run , 2001 .

[31]  A. Kemeny,et al.  Evaluating perception in driving simulation experiments , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  L. R. Young,et al.  Influence of combined visual and vestibular cues on human perception and control of horizontal rotation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Frank M. Cardullo,et al.  Developments in Human Centered Cueing Algorithms for Control of Flight Simulator Motion Systems , 1999 .

[34]  J. Lackner,et al.  Vertical linear self-motion perception during visual and inertial motion: more than weighted summation of sensory inputs. , 2005, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[35]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human visual orientation , 1982 .

[36]  Andras Kemeny,et al.  Motion Cueing in the Renault Driving Simulator , 2000 .

[37]  Christian Darlot,et al.  Using sensory weighting to model the influence of canal, otolith and visual cues on spatial orientation and eye movements , 2002, Biological Cybernetics.

[38]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Visuovestibular perception of self-motion modeled as a dynamic optimization process , 2002, Biological Cybernetics.

[39]  R. McCrea,et al.  Neck proprioceptive inputs to primate vestibular nucleus neurons , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  F. A. M. Steen,et al.  Induction and impairment of saturated yaw and surge vection , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Herschel W. Leibowitz,et al.  The Human Senses in Flight , 1988 .

[42]  J. Lishman,et al.  The Autonomy of Visual Kinaesthesis , 1973, Perception.

[43]  A J Gundry,et al.  Thresholds of perception for periodic linear motion. , 1978, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[44]  Gundry Aj,et al.  Thresholds of perception for periodic linear motion. , 1978 .

[45]  I. Howard,et al.  Visually Induced Reorientation Illusions , 2001, Perception.

[46]  Daniel R. Berger Spectral texturing for real-time applications , 2003, SIGGRAPH '03.

[47]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Visual-Vestibular Interaction: Effects on Self-Motion Perception and Postural Control , 1978 .

[48]  Lee Dn,et al.  The optic flow field: the foundation of vision. , 1980 .

[49]  B J Frost,et al.  The effect of visual-vestibular conflict on the latency of steady-state visually induced subjective rotation , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[50]  Vincent Nougier,et al.  Perception of slow pitch and roll body tilts in bilateral labyrinthine-defective subjects , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  Dora E. Angelaki,et al.  Neurons compute internal models of the physical laws of motion , 2004, Nature.

[52]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Does the Quality of the Computer Graphics Matter when Judging Distances in Visually Immersive Environments? , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[53]  I. Israël,et al.  Multifactorial interactions involved in linear self-transport distance estimate: a place for time. , 2004, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[54]  H. Mittelstaedt A new solution to the problem of the subjective vertical , 1983, Naturwissenschaften.

[55]  G. Paige,et al.  Multiple sensory cues underlying the perception of translation and path. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  P. M. Jaekl,et al.  Perceiving a stable world during active rotational and translational head movements , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.