Visual search under scotopic lighting conditions
暂无分享,去创建一个
Wilson S. Geisler | Karl R. Gegenfurtner | Vivian C. Paulun | Alexander C. Schütz | Melchi M. Michel | W. Geisler | A. Schütz | K. Gegenfurtner
[1] Frank H Durgin,et al. Upper-left gaze bias reveals competing search strategies in a reverse Stroop task. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[2] Dennis R. Proffitt,et al. Within striking distance: Task efficacy influences perceived size and distance , 2010 .
[3] E. Peli,et al. Image invariance with changes in size: the role of peripheral contrast thresholds. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[4] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 3.0 , 1997 .
[5] Donald I. A. MacLeod,et al. Rod flicker perception: Scotopic duality, phase lags and destructive interference , 1989, Vision Research.
[6] Melchi M. Michel,et al. Intrinsic position uncertainty explains detection and localization performance in peripheral vision. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[7] P. Lennie,et al. Shared pathways for rod and cone vision , 1986, Vision Research.
[8] H. Levitt. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[9] J. Cohen,et al. Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulas , 1968 .
[10] D. Macleod,et al. Rods Cancel Cones in Flicker , 1972, Nature.
[11] D G Pelli,et al. Uncertainty explains many aspects of visual contrast detection and discrimination. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[12] Alexander C. Schütz,et al. Dynamic integration of information about salience and value for saccadic eye movements , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[13] Michael S Landy,et al. Choice of saccade endpoint under risk. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[14] Jeffrey S. Perry,et al. Visual search: the role of peripheral information measured using gaze-contingent displays. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[15] Laurence T. Maloney,et al. Human Visual Search Does Not Maximize the Post-Saccadic Probability of Identifying Targets , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[16] Matthew F. Peterson,et al. Looking just below the eyes is optimal across face recognition tasks , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[17] Matthew F. Peterson,et al. Individual Differences in Eye Movements During Face Identification Reflect Observer-Specific Optimal Points of Fixation , 2013, Psychological science.
[18] R. F. Hess,et al. The contrast sensitivity gradient across the human visual field: With emphasis on the low spatial frequency range , 1989, Vision Research.
[19] D. Burr,et al. Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements , 1994, Nature.
[20] Miguel P Eckstein,et al. Visual search: a retrospective. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[21] M. Eckstein,et al. Quantifying the Performance Limits of Human Saccadic Targeting during Visual Search , 2001, Perception.
[22] J. D. Gould,et al. Eye-Movement Patterns in Scanning Numeric Displays , 1965, Perceptual and motor skills.
[23] Preeti Verghese,et al. Feedback about gaze position improves saccade efficiency. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[24] F. Chédru,et al. Visual searching in normal and brain-damaged subjects (contribution to the study of unilateral inattention). , 1973, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.
[25] A. Stockman,et al. The spectral sensitivities of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive cones derived from measurements in observers of known genotype , 2000, Vision Research.
[26] F. Previc. Attentional and Oculomotor Influences on Visual Field Anisotropies in Visual Search Performance , 1996 .
[27] Preeti Verghese,et al. Active search for multiple targets is inefficient , 2010, Vision Research.
[28] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Simple summation rule for optimal fixation selection in visual search , 2009, Vision Research.
[29] Preeti Verghese,et al. Immediate feedback improves saccadic efficiency , 2013 .
[30] Lester C. Loschky,et al. Investigating spatial vision and dynamic attentional selection using a gaze-contingent multiresolutional display. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[31] Tae-Eui Kam,et al. An fMRI Study of Cortical Responses for Reflectance-specific Image Motion , 2013 .
[32] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Optimal eye movement strategies in visual search , 2005, Nature.
[33] F. Billmeyer. Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd ed., by Gunter Wyszecki and W. S. Stiles, John Wiley and Sons, New York, 1982, 950 pp. Price: $75.00 , 1983 .
[34] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 4.0: Current Progress With a Model of Visual Search , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.
[35] Chuan Yi Tang,et al. A 2.|E|-Bit Distributed Algorithm for the Directed Euler Trail Problem , 1993, Inf. Process. Lett..
[36] Iain D Gilchrist,et al. Target location probability effects in visual search: an effect of sequential dependencies. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] U. Neisser. VISUAL SEARCH. , 1964, Scientific American.
[38] Matthew F. Peterson,et al. Learning optimal eye movements to unusual faces , 2014, Vision Research.
[39] Gunther Wyszecki,et al. Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition , 2000 .
[40] G. Zelinsky. Using Eye Saccades to Assess the Selectivity of Search Movements , 1996, Vision Research.
[41] R. Hess,et al. Regional variation of contrast sensitivity across the retina of the achromat: sensitivity of human rod vision. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.
[42] A. Kramer,et al. Attentional guidance of the eyes by contextual information and abrupt onsets. , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[43] Jiri Najemnik,et al. Eye movement statistics in humans are consistent with an optimal search strategy. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[44] F A Wichmann,et al. Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .
[45] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.
[46] J. Findlay. Saccade Target Selection During Visual Search , 1997, Vision Research.
[47] Preeti Verghese,et al. Where to look next? Eye movements reduce local uncertainty. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[48] B. C. Motter,et al. The guidance of eye movements during active visual search , 1998, Vision Research.
[49] Sjoerd Stuit,et al. Suppressed images selectively affect perceptual dominance in binocular rivalry , 2011 .
[50] A. Watson,et al. Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[51] A. Hendrickson,et al. Human photoreceptor topography , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[52] M. Ernst,et al. The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[53] Alexander C. Schütz,et al. Interindividual differences in preferred directions of perceptual and motor decisions. , 2014, Journal of vision.