Social top-down response modulation (STORM): a model of the control of mimicry in social interaction

As a distinct feature of human social interactions, spontaneous mimicry has been widely investigated in the past decade. Research suggests that mimicry is a subtle and flexible social behavior which plays an important role for communication and affiliation. However, fundamental questions like why and how people mimic still remain unclear. In this paper, we evaluate past theories of why people mimic and the brain systems that implement mimicry in social psychology and cognitive neuroscience. By reviewing recent behavioral and neuroimaging studies on the control of mimicry by social signals, we conclude that the subtlety and sophistication of mimicry in social contexts reflect a social top-down response modulation (STORM) which increases one's social advantage and this mechanism is most likely implemented by medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). We suggest that this STORM account of mimicry is important for our understanding of social behavior and social cognition, and provides implications for future research in autism.

[1]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Contextual modulation of mirror and countermirror sensorimotor associations. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al.  Attention modulates the specificity of automatic imitation to human actors , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  L. Tiedens,et al.  Power moves: complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  R. Dolan,et al.  Brain Responses to the Acquired Moral Status of Faces , 2004, Neuron.

[5]  E. Renzi,et al.  Imitation and utilisation behaviour. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[6]  R. Hari,et al.  Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. , 2009, Physiological reviews.

[7]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Chapter 5 Human Mimicry , 2009 .

[8]  Harriet Over,et al.  Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. , 2009, Developmental science.

[9]  D. Heeger,et al.  Normal Movement Selectivity in Autism , 2010, Neuron.

[10]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  Thijs Verwijmeren,et al.  Mimicking Attractive Opposite-Sex Others: The Role of Romantic Relationship Status , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[12]  Marcel Brass,et al.  The Inhibition of Imitative Response Tendencies , 2001, NeuroImage.

[13]  L. Miles,et al.  Implicit Behavioral Mimicry: Investigating the Impact of Group Membership , 2006 .

[14]  M. Brass,et al.  Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Training social cognition: From imitation to Theory of Mind , 2012, Cognition.

[16]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[17]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Eye contact enhances mimicry of intransitive hand movements , 2011, Biology Letters.

[18]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System , 2007, Current Biology.

[19]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  The Role of Facial Mimicry in the Recognition of Affect , 2008, Psychological science.

[20]  Clare Press,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Action Observation and Robotic Agents: Learning and Anthropomorphism , 2022 .

[21]  A. Wohlschläger,et al.  The link between facial feedback and neural activity within central circuitries of emotion--new insights from botulinum toxin-induced denervation of frown muscles. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  M. Brass,et al.  Hyperimitation of Actions Is Related to Reduced Understanding of Others' Minds in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[23]  G. Csibra,et al.  Sensitivity to communicative relevance tells young children what to imitate. , 2009, Developmental science.

[24]  A. Whiten,et al.  A Systematic Review of Action Imitation in Autistic Spectrum Disorder , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[25]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  The simulating social mind: the role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  C. Frith,et al.  “Hey John”: Signals Conveying Communicative Intention toward the Self Activate Brain Regions Associated with “Mentalizing,” Regardless of Modality , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Joseph P. McCleery,et al.  EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[28]  D. Perrett,et al.  Imitation, mirror neurons and autism , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[29]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  R. Baaren,et al.  Mimicry for money: Behavioral consequences of imitation , 2003 .

[31]  C. Heyes Imitation: Associative and Context Dependent , 2013 .

[32]  W. Prinz,et al.  Compatibility between Observed and Executed Finger Movements: Comparing Symbolic, Spatial, and Imitative Cues , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[33]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Using Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry to Create Affiliation and Rapport , 2003, Psychological science.

[34]  Richard Ramsey,et al.  The Control of Mimicry by Eye Contact Is Mediated by Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  G. Csibra Action mirroring and action understanding: an alternative account , 1993 .

[36]  James Stanley,et al.  Effects of Agency on Movement Interference During Observation of a Moving Dot Stimulus , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Self-monitoring without awareness: using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  Paul W. Burgess,et al.  Abnormal functional specialization within medial prefrontal cortex in high-functioning autism: a multi-voxel similarity analysis , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  V. Gallese Before and below ‘theory of mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  Chameleons bake bigger pies and take bigger pieces: Strategic behavioral mimicry facilitates negotiation outcomes , 2007 .

[41]  G. Bird,et al.  Atypical Social Modulation of Imitation in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2011, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[42]  C. Zink,et al.  Know Your Place: Neural Processing of Social Hierarchy in Humans , 2008, Neuron.

[43]  Tanya L. Chartrand,et al.  I Am Too Just Like You , 2008, Psychological science.

[44]  Antonia F. de C. Hamilton,et al.  Dissociation of mirroring and mentalising systems in autism , 2011, NeuroImage.

[45]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[46]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Through the looking glass: counter‐mirror activation following incompatible sensorimotor learning , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[47]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Contextual Modulation of Mirror and Countermirror , 2012 .

[48]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Resisting motor mimicry: Control of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and temporo-parietal lesions , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[50]  E. Mohammadi,et al.  Barriers and facilitators related to the implementation of a physiological track and trigger system: A systematic review of the qualitative evidence , 2017, International journal for quality in health care : journal of the International Society for Quality in Health Care.

[51]  M. Brass,et al.  Investigating Action Understanding: Inferential Processes versus Action Simulation , 2007, Current Biology.

[52]  G. Csibra,et al.  Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals , 2008, Current Biology.

[53]  Cade McCall,et al.  Effects of a priori liking on the elicitation of mimicry. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[54]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[55]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Unbroken mirrors: challenging a theory of Autism , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  B. Pillon,et al.  Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[57]  James Stanley,et al.  How instructions modify perception: An fMRI study investigating brain areas involved in attributing human agency , 2010, NeuroImage.

[58]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  It takes two to mimic: behavioral consequences of self-construals. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[59]  M. Brass,et al.  Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Daniel P. Kennedy,et al.  Failing to deactivate: resting functional abnormalities in autism. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  The brain's intention to imitate: The neurobiology of intentional versus automatic imitation , 2008, Brain Stimulation.

[62]  Gregory Hickok,et al.  Eight Problems for the Mirror Neuron Theory of Action Understanding in Monkeys and Humans , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  Jan Derrfuss,et al.  Imitative response tendencies in patients with frontal brain lesions. , 2003, Neuropsychology.

[64]  A. Luria Higher Cortical Functions in Man , 1980, Springer US.

[65]  M. Pickering,et al.  Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[66]  C. Heyes What's social about social learning? , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[67]  F. Lhermitte,et al.  Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part II: Patient behavior in complex and social situations: The “environmental dependency syndrome” , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[68]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[70]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The Chameleon Effect as Social Glue: Evidence for the Evolutionary Significance of Nonconscious Mimicry , 2003 .

[71]  P. Hare Advance Online Publication , 2002, Nature Medicine.

[72]  C. Heyes Causes and consequences of imitation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[73]  V. Gallese Mirror Neurons, Embodied Simulation, and the Neural Basis of Social Identification , 2009 .

[74]  Paul Pauli,et al.  Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes , 2008 .

[75]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Where is the love? The social aspects of mimicry , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[76]  Cecilia Heyes,et al.  Evolution, development and intentional control of imitation , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[77]  R. Baaren,et al.  The Influence of Facial Attractiveness on Imitation , 2009 .

[78]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  Mimicry and Prosocial Behavior , 2004, Psychological science.

[79]  M. Brass,et al.  The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[80]  C. Heyes WHAT CAN IMITATION DO FOR COOPERATION ? , 2010 .

[81]  M. Bateson,et al.  Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting , 2006, Biology Letters.

[82]  J. Mattingley,et al.  The role of selective attention in matching observed and executed actions , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[83]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[84]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[85]  Sukhvinder S. Obhi,et al.  Resonating with Others: The Effects of Self-Construal Type on Motor Cortical Output , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[86]  M. Mermillod,et al.  Is eye contact the key to the social brain? , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[87]  U. Hess,et al.  The impact of social context on mimicry , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[88]  James M. Kilner,et al.  Learning to understand others' actions , 2010, Biology Letters.

[89]  A. Hamilton Emulation and Mimicry for Social Interaction: A Theoretical Approach to Imitation in Autism , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[90]  C. Heyes,et al.  Experience modulates automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[91]  P. Fletcher,et al.  Seeing other minds: attributed mental states influence perception , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[92]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[93]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Minimizing motor mimicry by myself: Self-focus enhances online action-control mechanisms during motor contagion , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[94]  C. Heyes Automatic imitation. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[95]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Social attitudes modulate automatic imitation , 2010 .