Ordinality and inferential abilities of a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus).

A grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), able to label the color of the bigger or smaller object in a pair (I. M. Pepperberg & M. V. Brezinsky, 1991), to vocally quantify < or =6 item sets (including heterogeneous subsets; I. M. Pepperberg, 1994), and separately trained to identify Arabic numerals 1-6 with the same vocal English labels but not to associate Arabic numbers with their relevant physical quantities, was shown pairs of Arabic numbers or an Arabic numeral and a set of objects and was asked for the color of the bigger or smaller one. The parrot's success showed he (a) understood number symbols as abstract representations of real-world collections, (b) inferred the relationship between the Arabic number and the quantity via stimulus equivalence, and (c) understood the ordinal relationship of his numbers.

[1]  I. Pepperberg,et al.  Acquisition of a relative class concept by an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus): discriminations based on relative size. , 1991, Journal of comparative psychology.

[2]  P Hineline,et al.  How, then, shall we characterize this elephant? , 1997, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[3]  Susan C. Levine,et al.  Quantitative Development in Infancy and Early Childhood , 2002 .

[4]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Cognitive Neuroscience: Numerate Neurons , 2003, Current Biology.

[5]  Avishai Henik,et al.  The development of internal representations of magnitude and their association with Arabic numerals. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The Organization of Brain Activations in Number Comparison: Event-Related Potentials and the Additive-Factors Method , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  J. E. Weaver,et al.  Symmetry training in pigeons can produce functional equivalences , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  C. B. Ferster The experimental analysis of human behavior. , 1963 .

[9]  David A. Washburn,et al.  Ordinal Judgments of Numerical Symbols by Macaques (Macaca Mulatta) , 1991, Psychological science.

[10]  R. Bruce,et al.  One, Two, Three and Counting , 2004 .

[11]  E. J. Capaldi,et al.  The Development of numerical competence : animal and human models , 1993 .

[12]  Takeshi Hatta,et al.  Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[13]  K. Gibson,et al.  “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives on ape “language” , 1990 .

[14]  M. Posner,et al.  Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  Jacky Emmerton,et al.  Pigeons’ serial ordering of numerosity with visual arrays , 1997 .

[16]  S Dehaene,et al.  Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  E. Wasserman,et al.  Superordinate categorization via learned stimulus equivalence: quantity of reinforcement, hedonic value, and the nature of the mediator. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[18]  G. Orban,et al.  Parietal Representation of Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Magnitude , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Child's Understanding of Number , 1979 .

[20]  B. Roche,et al.  Relational frame theory : A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition , 2004 .

[21]  D. Berch,et al.  Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  T. Simon,et al.  The foundations of numerical thinking in a brain without numbers , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  Irene M. Pepperberg,et al.  Object permanence in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) , 1986 .

[24]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Approximate quantities and exact number words: dissociable systems , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: The development of numerical skills in the chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) , 1990 .

[26]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[27]  Responses to quantity: Perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1995 .

[28]  H S Terrace,et al.  Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. , 1998, Science.

[29]  T. Matsuzawa,et al.  TEACHING ORDINALS TO A CARDINAL-TRAINED CHIMPANZEE , 1993 .

[30]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Response-Selection-Related Parietal Activation during Number Comparison , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  H S Terrace,et al.  Representation of the numerosities 1-9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[32]  J. Grafman,et al.  The calculating brain: an fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  J. Fetterman,et al.  The development of emergent differential sample behavior in pigeons. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[34]  G. Berntson,et al.  Responses to quantity: perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[35]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[36]  D J Navarick The experimental analysis of human behavior (Editorial). , 1990, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[37]  W. Vaughan,et al.  Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons , 1988 .

[38]  P. Urcuioli,et al.  Stimulus control topographies and tests of symmetry in pigeons. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[39]  Hiroshi Fukuda,et al.  A functional MRI study of simple arithmetic--a comparison between children and adults. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[40]  I. Pepperberg Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus) numerical abilities: addition and further experiments on a zero-like concept. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.

[41]  K. Fuson Children's Counting and Concepts of Number , 1987 .

[42]  C. Lowe,et al.  Naming and stimulus equivalence. , 1990 .

[43]  C. Lowe,et al.  Testing for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of language-trained chimpanzees. , 2000, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[44]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy , 2002, Cognition.

[45]  S. Hayes,et al.  OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION IN LANGUAGE-ABLE AND LANGUAGE-DISABLED CHILDREN , 2005 .

[46]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Biological foundations of numerical thinking Response to T.J. Simon (1999) , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  R. Schusterman,et al.  Equivalence classification by California sea lions using class-specific reinforcers. , 2001, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[48]  J. Delius,et al.  A new approach to the formation of equivalence classes in pigeons. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[49]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Differential Contributions of the Left and Right Inferior Parietal Lobules to Number Processing , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Biological foundations of numerical thinkingto T.J. Simon (1999) , 1999 .

[52]  rTMS disrupts the representation of small numbers in supramarginal gyrus , 2001, NeuroImage.

[53]  M. Harris Language Comprehension in Ape and Child , 1994 .

[54]  D. Rumbaugh,et al.  Do apes use language , 1980 .

[55]  I. Pepperberg,et al.  Number comprehension by a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), including a zero-like concept. , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.

[56]  W A Roberts,et al.  Summation of symbols by pigeons (Columba livia): the importance of number and mass of reward items. , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[57]  T. Zentall Symbolic Representation by Pigeons , 2000 .

[58]  A. Kappers Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution , 2022 .

[59]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[60]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Magnitude comparison in preschoolers: what counts? Influence of perceptual variables. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[61]  Dora Biro,et al.  Use of numerical symbols by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Cardinals, ordinals, and the introduction of zero , 2001, Animal Cognition.

[62]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Numerical and arithmetical cognition: a longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with learning disability. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[63]  Gerald E. Hough,et al.  Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[64]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Numerical processing in the two hemispheres: Studies of a split-brain patient , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[65]  O. Wirth,et al.  Stability of functional equivalence and stimulus equivalence: effects of baseline reversals. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[66]  L Girelli,et al.  The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[67]  Paul B. Buckley,et al.  Comparisons of digits and dot patterns. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[68]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Cognition: Numerical memory span in a chimpanzee , 2000, Nature.

[69]  William J. McIlvane,et al.  The Search for Stimulus Equivalence in Nonverbal Organisms , 1993 .

[70]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  The Mental Number Line and the Human Angular Gyrus , 2001, NeuroImage.

[71]  C F Lowe,et al.  On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. , 1996, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[72]  R. Fouts,et al.  Transfer of signed responses in American Sign Language from vocal English stimuli to physical object stimuli by a chimpanzee (Pan) , 1976 .

[73]  C F Lowe,et al.  Reflections on naming and other symbolic behavior. , 1996, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[74]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[75]  William A. Roberts,et al.  Judgments of ordinality and summation of number symbols by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). , 1997 .

[76]  Albert Yonas,et al.  Infants' Sensitivity to Relative Size Information for Distance. , 1985 .

[77]  Irene M. Pepperberg,et al.  Grey parrots do not always ‘parrot’: the roles of imitation and phonological awareness in the creation of new labels from existing vocalizations , 2007 .

[78]  I. Pepperberg The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots , 2000 .

[79]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Different cortical activations for subjects using allocentric or egocentric strategies in a virtual navigation task , 2004, Neuroreport.

[80]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Use of numbers by a chimpanzee , 1985, Nature.

[81]  I. Pepperberg Numerical competence in an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus). , 1994 .

[82]  G. Berntson,et al.  Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[83]  Colleen Reichmuth Kastak,et al.  Sea lions and equivalence: expanding classes by exclusion. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[84]  Sonia Ragir,et al.  “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes. Comparative developmental perspectives , 1991, International Journal of Primatology.

[85]  Ronald J. Schusterman,et al.  A California Sea Lion (Zalophus Californianus) is Capable of Forming Equivalence Relations , 1993 .

[86]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Language and number: a bilingual training study , 2001, Cognition.

[87]  R. Moyer Comparing objects in memory: Evidence suggesting an internal psychophysics , 1973 .

[88]  I. Pepperberg Cognition in an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Further evidence for comprehension of categories and labels. , 1990 .

[89]  E. Nakagawa Acquired Equivalence of Discriminative Stimuli Following Two Concurrent Discrimination Learning Tasks as a Function of Overtraining in Rats , 1999 .

[90]  Irene M. Pepperberg,et al.  Proficient performance of a conjunctive, recursive task by an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus). , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[91]  I. Pepperberg,et al.  Lack of referential vocal learning from LCD video by grey parrots , 2004 .

[92]  K. Wilkinson,et al.  Methods for Studying Symbolic Behavior and Category Formation: Contributions of Stimulus Equivalence Research , 2001 .

[93]  Middle-wave cone and rod visual pigments in birds: Spectral sensitivity and opsin structure , 1996 .

[94]  S. Carey Bootstrapping & the origin of concepts , 2004, Daedalus.

[95]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[96]  Peter G Judge,et al.  Ordinal representation of numeric quantities by brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[97]  R K Thomas,et al.  Conceptual numerousness judgments by squirrel monkeys. , 1980, The American journal of psychology.

[98]  David Premack,et al.  Intelligence In Ape And Man , 1976 .

[99]  R. Schusterman,et al.  Functional equivalence in a California sea lion: relevance to animal social and communicative interactions , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[100]  M Sidman,et al.  Functional classes and equivalence relations. , 1989, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[101]  Catherine Sophian,et al.  Children’s Numbers , 1994 .

[102]  H. Davis,et al.  Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[103]  E. Miller,et al.  Coding of Cognitive Magnitude Compressed Scaling of Numerical Information in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[104]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[105]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale , 2001, Psychological science.

[106]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Arithmetic and the Brain This Review Comes from a Themed Issue on Cognitive Neuroscience Edited the Intraparietal Sulcus and Number Sense Number Sense in the Animal Brain , 2022 .

[107]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language and calculation within the parietal lobe: a combined cognitive, anatomical and fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[108]  L. Fersen,et al.  Acquired equivalences between auditory stimuli in dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) , 2000, Animal Cognition.

[109]  Sarah T. Boysen,et al.  Primate numerical competence: contributions toward understanding nonhuman cognition , 2000, Cogn. Sci..

[110]  G. Green,et al.  Development of conditional and equivalence relations without differential consequences. , 1990, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[111]  Sarah T. Boysen,et al.  Counting in Chimpanzees: Nonhuman Principles and Emergent Properties of Number , 1993 .

[112]  C. Sophian From objects to quantities: developments in preschool children's judgments about aggregate amount. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[113]  G. Berntson,et al.  Processing of ordinality and transitivity by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.

[114]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[115]  Silke M Göbel,et al.  Cognitive Neuroscience: Acting on Numbers , 2004, Current Biology.