Environmental Design Shapes Perceptual-motor Exploration, Learning, and Transfer in Climbing

This study investigated how environmental design shapes perceptual-motor exploration, when meta-stable regions of performance are created. Here, we examined how creating meta-stable regions of performance could destabilize pre-existing skills, favoring greater exploration of performance environments, exemplified in this study by climbing surfaces. In this investigation we manipulated hold orientations on an indoor climbing wall to examine how nine climbers explored, learned, and transferred various trunk-rolling motion patterns and hand grasping movements. The learning protocol consisted of four sessions, in which climbers randomly ascended three different routes, as fluently as possible. All three routes were 10.3 m in height and composed of 20 hand-holds at the same locations on an artificial climbing wall; only hold orientations were altered: (i) a horizontal-edge route was designed to afford horizontal hold grasping, (ii) a vertical-edge route afforded vertical hold grasping, and (iii), a double-edge route was designed to afford both horizontal and vertical hold grasping. As a meta-stable condition of performance invite an individual to both exploit his pre-existing behavioral repertoire (i.e., horizontal hold grasping pattern and trunk face to the wall) and explore new behaviors (i.e., vertical hold grasping and trunk side to the wall), it was hypothesized that the double-edge route characterized a meta-stable region of performance. Data were collected from inertial measurement units located on the neck and hip of each climber, allowing us to compute rolling motion referenced to the artificial climbing wall. Information on ascent duration, the number of exploratory and performatory movements for locating hand-holds, and hip path was also observed in video footage from a frontal camera worn by participants. Climbing fluency was assessed by calculating geometric index of entropy. Results showed that the meta-stable condition of performance may have afforded utilization of more adaptive climbing behaviors (expressed in higher values for range and variability of trunk rolling motion and greater number of exploratory movements). Findings indicated how climbers learn to explore and, subsequently, use effective exploratory search strategies that can facilitate transfer of learning to performance in novel climbing environments.

[1]  K. M. Newell,et al.  Searching for solutions to the coordination function : learning as exploratory behavior , 1992 .

[2]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  Inexperienced Sport Climbers Might Perceive and Utilize New Opportunities for Action by Merely Observing a Model , 2002, Perceptual and motor skills.

[3]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[4]  Patrick Cordier,et al.  Harmonic analysis of a complex motor behavior , 1996 .

[5]  Romain Hérault,et al.  Climbing skill and complexity of climbing wall design: assessment of jerk as a novel indicator of performance fluency. , 2014, Journal of applied biomechanics.

[6]  Keith Davids,et al.  Temporal dynamics of inter-limb coordination in ice climbing revealed through change-point analysis of the geodesic mean of circular data , 2013 .

[7]  Keith Davids,et al.  Dynamics of movement patterning in learning a discrete multiarticular action. , 2008, Motor control.

[8]  K. Davids,et al.  Neurobiological Degeneracy and Affordance Perception Support Functional Intra-Individual Variability of Inter-Limb Coordination during Ice Climbing , 2014, PloS one.

[9]  Romain Hérault,et al.  A dynamical system perspective to understanding badminton singles game play. , 2014, Human movement science.

[10]  Philippe Bolon,et al.  Entropy, degrees of freedom, and free climbing: a thermodynamic study of a complex behavior based on trajectory analysis. , 1993 .

[11]  Robert D. Martin,et al.  The Behavioural Physiology of Animals and Man. , 1974 .

[12]  K. Davids,et al.  What exactly is acquired during skill acquisition , 2011 .

[13]  Armin Fuchs,et al.  Beyond the blank slate: routes to learning new coordination patterns depend on the intrinsic dynamics of the learner—experimental evidence and theoretical model , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  S. Bennett,et al.  Extended Book Review: Dynamics of Skill Acquisition: A Constraints-Led Approach , 2007 .

[15]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  The Perceptual-Motor Workspace and the Acquisition of Skill , 1991 .

[16]  M. Kringelbach Learning to Change , 2004, PLoS biology.

[17]  J. Jacques Temprado,et al.  A novice-expert comparison of (intra-limb) coordination subserving the volleyball serve , 1997 .

[18]  P. N. Kugler,et al.  Search Strategies and the Acquisition of Coordination , 1989 .

[19]  W. B. Quay The Behavioural Physiology of Animals and Man: The Collected Papers of Erich Von Holst, Vol. 1 Robert Martin Erich von Holst , 1974 .

[20]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[21]  K. Newell,et al.  Dimensional change in motor learning. , 2001, Human movement science.

[22]  Karl M Newell,et al.  Transfer as a function of exploration and stabilization in original practice. , 2015, Human movement science.

[23]  Jean Pailhous,et al.  Harmonic analysis of a complex , 1996 .

[24]  Philippe Bolon,et al.  Entropy as a global variable of the learning process , 1994 .

[25]  Sean Müller,et al.  A Model for the Transfer of Perceptual-Motor Skill Learning in Human Behaviors , 2012, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[26]  Sebastian O. H. Madgwick,et al.  An efficient orientation filter for inertial and inertial / magnetic sensor arrays , 2010 .

[27]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Brain coordination dynamics: True and false faces of phase synchrony and metastability , 2009, Progress in Neurobiology.

[28]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  The Role of Anxiety in Perceiving and Realizing Affordances , 2006 .

[29]  Sebastian Madgwick,et al.  Estimation of IMU and MARG orientation using a gradient descent algorithm , 2011, 2011 IEEE International Conference on Rehabilitation Robotics.

[30]  J A Scott Kelso,et al.  An Essay on Understanding the Mind , 2008, Ecological psychology : a publication of the International Society for Ecological Psychology.

[31]  Didier Delignières,et al.  A psychophysical study of difficulty rating in rock climbing. , 1993 .

[32]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[33]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: collective and component levels. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Keith Davids,et al.  Affordance-controlled bifurcations of action patterns in martial arts. , 2006, Nonlinear dynamics, psychology, and life sciences.

[35]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic coordination in the brain , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  Keith Davids,et al.  How boxers decide to punch a target: emergent behaviour in nonlinear dynamical movement systems. , 2006, Journal of sports science & medicine.

[37]  X Sanchez,et al.  Efficacy of pre‐ascent climbing route visual inspection in indoor sport climbing , 2012, Scandinavian journal of medicine & science in sports.

[38]  Romain Hérault,et al.  Pattern Recognition in Cyclic and Discrete Skills Performance from Inertial Measurement Units , 2014 .

[39]  D. Araújo,et al.  Affordances can invite behavior: Reconsidering the relationship between affordances and agency , 2012 .

[40]  Kevin C. Phillips,et al.  Optimizing Rock Climbing Performance Through Sport-Specific Strength and Conditioning , 2012 .

[41]  K. Davids,et al.  Ecological dynamics and motor learning design in sport , 2012 .

[42]  C. Assaiante,et al.  A statistical approach to sensorimotor strategies: conjugate cross-correlations. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[43]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer across different effector systems. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  R G Eston,et al.  Statistical analyses in the physiology of exercise and kinanthropometry , 2001, Journal of sports sciences.

[45]  K. Newell Motor skill acquisition. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[46]  K. Davids,et al.  Information for regulating action in sport: Metastability and emergence of tactical solutions under ecological constraints , 2009 .

[47]  G. G. Stokes "J." , 1890, The New Yale Book of Quotations.

[48]  P G Zanone,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  Didier Delignières,et al.  The nature of the transition between novice and skilled coordination during learning to swing. , 2007, Human movement science.

[50]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Constraints on the Development of Coordination , 1986 .

[51]  M. Turvey,et al.  Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. , 2009 .

[52]  Guang-Zhong Yang,et al.  ClimBSN: Climber performance monitoring with BSN , 2008, 2008 5th International Summer School and Symposium on Medical Devices and Biosensors.

[53]  J. Kelso,et al.  The Complementary Nature , 2006 .

[54]  J. van der Kamp,et al.  Towards a new ecological conception of perceptual information: lessons from a developmental systems perspective. , 2010, Human movement science.

[55]  Keith Davids,et al.  Metastability and emergent performance of dynamic interceptive actions. , 2012, Journal of science and medicine in sport.

[56]  G. Edelman,et al.  Solving Bernstein's problem: a proposal for the development of coordinated movement by selection. , 1993, Child development.

[57]  Claire F. Michaels,et al.  Direct Learning , 2007 .

[58]  S. R. Jammalamadaka,et al.  Directional Statistics, I , 2011 .

[59]  J. Kelso,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .

[60]  Elise Faugloire,et al.  (De)stabilization of required and spontaneous postural dynamics with learning. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  Cassim Ladha,et al.  ClimbAX: skill assessment for climbing enthusiasts , 2013, UbiComp.

[62]  L Seifert,et al.  Skill transfer, affordances and dexterity in different climbing environments. , 2013, Human movement science.

[63]  N. A. Bernstein,et al.  The Primacy of Action in Development , 2014 .

[64]  Gavin Blackwell,et al.  Self-reported ability assessment in rock climbing , 2011, Journal of sports sciences.

[65]  K. Newell Coordination, Control and Skill , 1985 .

[66]  J. Temprado,et al.  Covariation of attentional cost and stability provides further evidence for two routes to learning new coordination patterns. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[67]  J. Kelso,et al.  Learning as change of coordination dynamics: theory and experiment. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[68]  I Frosio,et al.  3D analysis of the body center of mass in rock climbing. , 2007, Human movement science.

[69]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[70]  Didier Delignières,et al.  On Discontinuities in Motor Learning: A Longitudinal Study of Complex Skill Acquisition on a Ski-Simulator , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.