Effects of suppressing the urge to drink on the accessibility of alcohol outcome expectancies.

Previous work has shown that attempts to deliberately suppress a given thought is associated with heightened accessibility of thought-related information both during and following suppression (Wegner, 1994, Psychological Review, 101, 34-52). This study examined whether attempts to suppress the urge for alcohol would similarly be associated with heightened accessibility of alcohol-related information. Heavy social drinkers were exposed to the sight and smell of their usual alcoholic beverage either under the instructions to suppress their urge to drink alcohol or without such instruction. Following this task, participants were asked to make timed judgements about the applicability of a series of alcohol outcome expectancies. Results supported the view that suppression increases the accessibility of information in memory. Those in the Suppression condition were faster to endorse alcohol outcome expectancies following the exposure to alcohol cues than those in the Control condition. Findings are discussed in terms of cognitive strategies for regulating alcohol use and patterns of restrained drinking.

[1]  M. Goldman,et al.  Alcohol-related expectancies versus demographic/background variables in the prediction of adolescent drinking. , 1983, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[2]  M. Kozak,et al.  Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  M. Goldman,et al.  Substance Use Disorders: Cognitive Models and Architectures , 1993 .

[4]  D. Clark,et al.  An experimental investigation of thought suppression. , 1991, Behaviour research and therapy.

[5]  B. Jones,et al.  Negative and positive alcohol expectancies as predictors of abstinence after discharge from a residential treatment program: a one-month and three-month follow-up study in men. , 1994, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[6]  M. Goldman,et al.  Implicit Priming of Alcohol Expectancy Memory Processes and Subsequent Drinking Behavior , 1995 .

[7]  D J Rohsenow,et al.  Relevance of cue reactivity to understanding alcohol and smoking relapse. , 1988, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[8]  D. Wegner Ironic processes of mental control. , 1994, Psychological review.

[9]  Philip C. Kendall,et al.  Psychopathology and cognition , 1993 .

[10]  J. E. Sherman,et al.  The motivation to use drugs: a psychobiological analysis of urges. , 1986, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[11]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  Kenneth E. Leonard,et al.  Psychological Theories of Drinking and Alcoholism , 1986 .

[13]  B. Leigh,et al.  Memory accessibility and association of alcohol use and its positive outcomes. , 1994 .

[14]  J. Polivy,et al.  Mental control of eating: Excitatory and inhibitory food thoughts. , 1993 .

[15]  T. Oei,et al.  Expectancy theory: a two-process model of alcohol use and abuse. , 1994, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[16]  M. Goldman,et al.  Drinking-related differences in the memory organization of alcohol expectancies. , 1994 .

[17]  M. Goldman,et al.  Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy memory network using multidimensional scaling. , 1992, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  A. Bandura Social learning theory , 1977 .

[19]  P. Salkovskis,et al.  Thought suppression and smoking cessation. , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[20]  D. Abrams,et al.  Cue elicited urge to drink and salivation in alcoholics: Relationship to individual differences , 1992 .

[21]  D M Wegner,et al.  Depression and mental control: the resurgence of unwanted negative thoughts. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  T. Palfai,et al.  Effects of suppressing the urge to drink on smoking topography: A preliminary study. , 1997 .

[23]  D. Cioffi,et al.  Delayed costs of suppressed pain. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  D J Rohsenow,et al.  Alcohol cue reactivity: effects of detoxification and extended exposure. , 1993, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[25]  Drinking habits and expectancies about alcohol's effects for self versus others. , 1983, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[26]  A. Ruderman,et al.  The development of a restrained drinking scale: a test of the abstinence violation effect among alcohol users. , 1984, Addictive behaviors.

[27]  Frank R. Kardes,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986 .

[28]  M. Goldman,et al.  Using alcohol expectancies to predict adolescent drinking behavior after one year. , 1989, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[29]  The suppression of exciting thoughts. , 1990 .

[30]  James W. Pennebaker,et al.  The Handbook of Mental Control , 1992 .

[31]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound , 1994 .

[32]  M. L. Cooper,et al.  Coping, expectancies, and alcohol abuse: a test of social learning formulations. , 1988, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[33]  G A Marlatt,et al.  Expectancy models of alcohol use. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  G. Connors,et al.  Drinking outcome expectancies among male alcoholics during relapse situations. , 1988, British journal of addiction.

[35]  S Shiffman,et al.  Coping with temptations to smoke. , 1984, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[36]  N. Heather,et al.  To drink or not to drink? Assessing conflicting desires in dependent drinkers in treatment. , 1993, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[37]  Daniel M. Wegner,et al.  The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. , 1992 .

[38]  C. Steele,et al.  Alcohol-related expectancies: defined by phase of intoxication and drinking experience. , 1981, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[39]  L. Roemer,et al.  Effects of suppressing thoughts about emotional material. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[40]  S. Tiffany,et al.  A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. , 1990, Psychological review.

[41]  L. Bensley Construct validity evidence for the interpretation of drinking restraint as a response conflict. , 1991, Addictive behaviors.