To transfer or not to transfer? Kinematics and laterality quotient predict interlimb transfer of motor learning.

Humans can remarkably adapt their motor behavior to novel environmental conditions, yet it remains unclear which factors enable us to transfer what we have learned with one limb to the other. Here we tested the hypothesis that interlimb transfer of sensorimotor adaptation is determined by environmental conditions but also by individual characteristics. We specifically examined the adaptation of unconstrained reaching movements to a novel Coriolis, velocity-dependent force field. Right-handed subjects sat at the center of a rotating platform and performed forward reaching movements with the upper limb toward flashed visual targets in prerotation, per-rotation (i.e., adaptation), and postrotation tests. Here only the dominant arm was used during adaptation and interlimb transfer was assessed by comparing performance of the nondominant arm before and after dominant-arm adaptation. Vision and no-vision conditions did not significantly influence interlimb transfer of trajectory adaptation, which on average was significant but limited. We uncovered a substantial heterogeneity of interlimb transfer across subjects and found that interlimb transfer can be qualitatively and quantitatively predicted for each healthy young individual. A classifier showed that in our study, interlimb transfer could be predicted based on the subject's task performance, most notably motor variability during learning, and his or her laterality quotient. Positive correlations suggested that variability of motor performance and lateralization of arm movement control facilitate interlimb transfer. We further show that these individual characteristics can predict the presence and the magnitude of interlimb transfer of left-handers. Overall, this study suggests that individual characteristics shape the way the nervous system can generalize motor learning.

[1]  J. Lackner,et al.  Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  J. Saunders,et al.  Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Ahmed A. Mostafa,et al.  Intermanual transfer and proprioceptive recalibration following training with translated visual feedback of the hand , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Amy J. Bastian,et al.  Inter- and intra-limb generalization of adaptation during catching , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  D C Noll,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of motor acquisition and motor transfer. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  R. Ivry,et al.  Ipsilateral motor cortex activity during unimanual hand movements relates to task complexity. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  M. Kawato,et al.  Visual Feedback Is Not Necessary for the Learning of Novel Dynamics , 2007, PloS one.

[8]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Limitations in interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  S G Kim,et al.  Functional activation in motor cortex reflects the direction and the degree of handedness. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Specificity of Reflex Adaptation for Task-Relevant Variability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  C. Grady,et al.  The Importance of Being Variable , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Ivry,et al.  Trial-by-trial analysis of intermanual transfer during visuomotor adaptation. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Eugene Poh,et al.  New visuomotor maps are immediately available to the opposite limb. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  A Schnitzler,et al.  Handedness and asymmetry of hand representation in human motor cortex. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  Force-field adaptation without proprioception: Can vision be used to model limb dynamics? , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  D. G. Woolley,et al.  Asymmetric interlimb transfer of concurrent adaptation to opposing dynamic forces , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  E. Vaadia,et al.  Differences in Context and Feedback Result in Different Trajectories and Adaptation Strategies in Reaching , 2009, PloS one.

[18]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Convergent models of handedness and brain lateralization , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[19]  S. Kinomura,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow changes of cortical motor areas and prefrontal areas in humans related to ipsilateral and contralateral hand movement , 1993, Brain Research.

[20]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  M. Hallett,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of ipsilateral motor cortex activation during unimanual motor tasks: further evidence for motor dominance , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[22]  M. Brainard,et al.  Performance variability enables adaptive plasticity of ‘crystallized’ adult birdsong , 2007, Nature.

[23]  D. Harrington,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of movement , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[24]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Interlimb transfer of novel inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Motor learning principles for neurorehabilitation. , 2013, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[26]  Yohsuke R. Miyamoto,et al.  Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  D Kilshaw,et al.  Right- and left-hand skill I: Effects of age, sex and hand preference showing superior skill in left-handers. , 1983, British journal of psychology.

[28]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Generalization in vision and motor control , 2004, Nature.

[29]  M M Cohen Visual Feedback, Distribution of Practice, and Intermanual Transfer of Prism Aftereffects , 1973, Perceptual and motor skills.

[30]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Motor variability is not noise, but grist for the learning mill , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Rachael Seidler,et al.  Degree of handedness affects intermanual transfer of skill learning , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Pratik K. Mutha,et al.  The Effects of Brain Lateralization on Motor Control and Adaptation , 2012, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  Wilsaan M Joiner,et al.  The training schedule affects the stability, not the magnitude, of the interlimb transfer of learned dynamics. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Joaquin A. Anguera,et al.  Neural correlates associated with intermanual transfer of sensorimotor adaptation , 2007, Brain Research.

[36]  Richard Chin,et al.  Principles and Practice of Clinical Trial Medicine , 2008 .

[37]  J. Vercher,et al.  Online control of the direction of rapid reaching movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Jordan A Taylor,et al.  Feedback-dependent generalization. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  G. Rees,et al.  The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Motor Task Variation Induces Structural Learning , 2009, Current Biology.

[41]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  D Timmann,et al.  Adaptation to visuomotor rotation and force field perturbation is correlated to different brain areas in patients with cerebellar degeneration. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  H. Imamizu,et al.  The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  David J Ostry,et al.  Modifiability of generalization in dynamics learning. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  M. Hinder,et al.  The contribution of visual feedback to visuomotor adaptation: How much and when? , 2008, Brain Research.

[46]  Britne A. Shabbott,et al.  Learning a visuomotor rotation: simultaneous visual and proprioceptive information is crucial for visuomotor remapping , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms , 2005 .

[49]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  Rajiv Ranganathan,et al.  Changing Up the Routine: Intervention-Induced Variability in Motor Learning , 2013, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[51]  D. Ostry,et al.  Stimulation of the Posterior Parietal Cortex Interferes with Arm Trajectory Adjustments during the Learning of New Dynamics , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. , 1993, Science.

[54]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Dissociable effects of the implicit and explicit memory systems on learning control of reaching , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  S. Kitazawa,et al.  Prism Adaptation of Reaching Movements: Specificity for the Velocity of Reaching , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Aaron S. Andalman,et al.  Vocal Experimentation in the Juvenile Songbird Requires a Basal Ganglia Circuit , 2005, PLoS biology.

[57]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor system , 2014, NeuroImage.

[58]  G. Deco,et al.  Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[59]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  D F Stegeman,et al.  Generalization and transfer of contextual cues in motor learning. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  C. S. Harris Adaptation to Displaced Vision: Visual, Motor, or Proprioceptive Change? , 1963, Science.