Time perspective and attitude-behaviour consistency in future-oriented behaviours.

The authors propose that the salience of a distant-future time perspective, compared to a near-future time perspective, should increase attitude-behaviour and attitude-intention consistency for future-oriented behaviours. To test this prediction, time perspective was experimentally manipulated in three studies. Across studies, participants in the distant-future time perspective condition demonstrated a strong positive relationship between attitudes towards future-oriented behaviours (saving and environmental protection) and corresponding intentions, as well as between attitudes and behaviour. In the near-future time perspective condition, the relationship between attitudes and intentions and attitudes and behaviour was significantly weaker than in the distant-future time perspective condition. The theoretical implications of these results and suggestions for future research are discussed.

[1]  P. Sheeran,et al.  Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behaviour relations: a meta-analysis of properties of variables from the theory of planned behaviour. , 2004, The British journal of social psychology.

[2]  Mark Snyder,et al.  When actions reflect attitudes: The politics of impression management , 1976 .

[3]  M. R. Leippe,et al.  When motives clash: Issue involvement and response involvement as determinants of persuasion. , 1987 .

[4]  R. Petty,et al.  Elaboration as a Determinant of Attitude Strength: Creating Attitudes That Are Persistent, Resistant, and Predictive of Behavior , 1995 .

[5]  Icek Ajzen,et al.  The directive influence of attitudes on behavior. , 1996 .

[6]  C. F. Kao,et al.  Central and peripheral routes to persuasion: An individual difference perspective. , 1986 .

[7]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Construal levels and self-control. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  P. Lunt Discourses of savings , 1996 .

[9]  Carol J. Williams,et al.  The Role of Attitude Accessibility in the Attitude-to-Behavior Process , 1989 .

[10]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Memory and Cognition in Its Social Context , 1989 .

[12]  P. Lunt,et al.  The relationship between involvement, attitude accessibility and attitude–behaviour consistency , 1997 .

[13]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  On the consistency between attitudes and behavior: Look to the method of attitude formation , 1977 .

[14]  R. LaPiere Attitudes vs Actions. 1934. , 1934, International journal of epidemiology.

[15]  Charles S. Carver,et al.  Physical aggression as a function of objective self-awareness and attitudes toward punishment , 1975 .

[16]  P. Sheeran,et al.  Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Anomalies in Intertemporal Choice: Evidence and an Interpretation , 1992 .

[18]  Jon Elster,et al.  Ulysses and the Sirens: A theory of imperfect rationality , 1977 .

[19]  Raymond De Young,et al.  Some Psychological Aspects of RecyclingThe Structure of Conservation - Satisfactions , 1986 .

[20]  P. Warr,et al.  Copyright © The British Psychological Society Unauthorised use and reproduction in any form (including the internet and other electronic means) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society. , 2005 .

[21]  E. Nyhus Psychological determinants of household saving behaviour , 2002 .

[22]  P. Webley,et al.  Life-cycle and dispositional routes into problem debt. , 2001, British journal of psychology.

[23]  M. Conner,et al.  Conscientiousness and the intention-behavior relationship: predicting exercise behavior. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[24]  J. Sivacek,et al.  Vested interest as a moderator of attitude–behavior consistency. , 1982 .

[25]  Paul C. Stern,et al.  Effect of incentives and education on resource conservation decisions in a simulated common dilemma. , 1976 .

[26]  R. Fazio On the power and functionality of attitudes: The role of attitude accessibility. , 1989 .

[27]  Jeff Joireman,et al.  Fiscal responsibility and the consideration of future consequences , 2005 .

[28]  Matthew W. Johnson,et al.  Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[29]  D. Boninger,et al.  The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. , 1994 .

[30]  A. W. Wicker Attitudes Versus Actions: The Relationship of Verbal and Overt Behavioral Responses to Attitude Objects. , 1969 .

[31]  P. Zimbardo,et al.  Putting Time in Perspective: A Valid, Reliable Individual-Differences Metric , 1999 .

[32]  D. Albarracín,et al.  Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: a meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  Philip G. Zimbardo,et al.  Time Perspective, Health, and Risk Taking. , 2006 .

[34]  F. Buttel NEW DIRECTIONS IN ENVIRONMENTAL SOCIOLOGY , 1987 .

[35]  Jennifer L. Perry,et al.  The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse , 2008, Psychopharmacology.

[36]  James M. Olson,et al.  Attitude Importance as a Function of Repeated Attitude Expression , 1994 .

[37]  M. Diehl,et al.  Effects of Attitudinal Ambivalence on Information Processing and Attitude-Intention Consistency☆ , 1997 .

[38]  R. Baumeister,et al.  High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. , 2004, Journal of personality.

[39]  A. Tesser,et al.  The effects of affective-cognitive consistency and thought on the attitude-behavior relation , 1989 .

[40]  P. Webley,et al.  Filling the gap between planning and doing: Psychological factors involved in the successful implementation of saving intention , 2007 .

[41]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Time Discounting and Time Preference: A Critical Review , 2002 .

[42]  B. Reynolds A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to drug use and gambling , 2006, Behavioural pharmacology.

[43]  R. Norman,et al.  Affective-cognitive consistency, attitudes, conformity, and behavior. , 1975 .

[44]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[45]  Christina Knussen,et al.  “I'm Not in the Habit of Recycling” , 2008 .

[46]  S. West,et al.  Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. , 1994 .

[47]  R. Thaler,et al.  An Economic Theory of Self-Control , 1977, Journal of Political Economy.

[48]  R. Pollak,et al.  SECOND-BEST NATIONAL SAVING AND GAME-EQUILIBRIUM GROWTH , 1980 .

[49]  Howard Rachlin,et al.  Self-control: Beyond commitment. , 1995 .

[50]  R. Fazio,et al.  Attitude accessibility as a moderator of the attitude-perception and attitude-behavior relations: an investigation of the 1984 presidential election. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  Stephan Meier,et al.  Impatience and Credit Behavior: Evidence from a Field Experiment , 2007 .

[52]  Arnold H. Buss,et al.  Self-consciousness, self-report of aggressiveness, and aggression , 1978 .

[53]  W. Crano,et al.  Attitudes and persuasion. , 2006, Annual review of psychology.

[54]  M. Zanna,et al.  Attitude–behavior consistency: An individual difference perspective. , 1980 .

[55]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. , 1977 .

[56]  Raymond De Young,et al.  SOME PSYCHOLOGICAL ASPECTS OF RECYCLING , 2011 .

[57]  A. Tesser,et al.  The role of beliefs and feelings in guiding behavior: The mismatch model , 1992 .

[58]  P. Sheeran,et al.  Temporal stability as a moderator of relationships in the Theory of Planned Behaviour. , 2000, The British journal of social psychology.

[59]  Icek Ajzen,et al.  From Intentions to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior , 1985 .