Effects of extinction with a compound conditioned stimulus
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Harold Gulliksen,et al. A theory of learning and transfer: I , 1938 .
[2] W. Estes,et al. Acquisition and transfer in pattern-vs.-component discrimination learning. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.
[3] Morton P. Friedman,et al. Transfer effects in discrimination learning , 1964 .
[4] S. J. Weiss. SUMMATION OF RESPONSE STRENGTHS INSTRUMENTALLY CONDITIONED TO STIMULI IN DIFFERENT SENSORY MODALITIES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.
[5] R. Rescorla. Reduction in the effectiveness of reinforcement after prior excitatory conditioning , 1970 .
[6] N. J. Mackintosh,et al. An Analysis of Overshadowing and Blocking , 1971 .
[7] R. Rescorla,et al. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .
[8] W. F. Prokasy,et al. Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .
[9] P. W. Frey,et al. Inhibition and learning , 1973 .
[10] William K. Estes,et al. Conditioning and behavior theory , 1975 .
[11] P. W. Frey,et al. Model of conditioning incorporating the Rescorla-Wagner associative axiom, a dynamic attention process, and a catastrophe rule. , 1978 .
[12] E. Kremer. The Rescorla-Wagner model: losses in associative strength in compound conditioned stimuli. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[13] N. Mackintosh,et al. One-Trial Overshadowing , 1979 .
[14] A. R. Wagner,et al. One-trial overshadowing: evidence of distributive processing. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[15] J. Pearce,et al. A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980 .
[16] W. Bellingham,et al. Spontaneous configuring to a tone-light compound using appetitive training , 1981 .
[17] T. Nicholas,et al. Blocking of inhibitory conditioning within a serial conditioned stimulus-conditioned inhibitor compound: Maintenance of acquired behavior without an unconditioned stimulus , 1983 .
[18] J. Pearce,et al. The Influence of Generalization Decrement on the Outcome of a Feature-Positive Discrimination , 1984 .
[19] E. Kehoe,et al. Summation and configuration in patterning schedules with the rat and rabbit , 1985 .
[20] E. Kehoe. Summation and configuration in conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response to compound stimuli. , 1986 .
[21] J. Pearce. A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. , 1987, Psychological review.
[22] J. Pearce,et al. Interactions between Conditioned Stimuli for Food and Water in the Rat , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[23] G. Bower,et al. From conditioning to category learning: an adaptive network model. , 1988 .
[24] E. Kehoe. A layered network model of associative learning: learning to learn and configuration. , 1988, Psychological review.
[25] J. Pearce,et al. A Role for Stimulus Generalization in Conditional Discrimination Learning , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[26] N. Mackintosh,et al. An associative theory of the representation of stimuli: Applications to perceptual learning and latent inhibition. , 1989 .
[27] R. Rescorla. Redundant treatment of neutral and excitatory stimuli in autoshaping. , 1989 .
[28] J. Pearce,et al. Configural associations in discrimination learning. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[29] R. Morris. Parallel Distributed Processing: Implications for Psychology and Neurobiology , 1990 .
[30] Feature-positive discrimination learning. , 1990 .
[31] J. Pearce,et al. Selective Transfer of Responding in Conditional Discriminations , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.