Unaccusativity in Sentence Production

Linguistic analyses suggest that there are two types of intransitive verbs: unaccusatives, whose sole argument is a patient or theme (e.g., fall), and unergatives, whose sole argument is an agent (e.g., jump).1 Past psycholinguistic experiments suggest that this distinction affects how sentences are processed: for example, it modulates both comprehension processes (Bever and Sanz 1997, Friedmann et al. 2008) and production processes (Kegl 1995, Kim 2006, M. Lee and Thompson 2004, J. Lee and Thompson 2011, McAllister et al. 2009). Given this body of evidence, it is reasonable to assume, as we do here, that this distinction is directly relevant to psycholinguistic theorizing. However, especially in production, exactly how this distinction affects processing is unknown, beyond the suggestion that unaccusatives somehow involve more complex processing than unergatives (see J. Lee and Thompson 2011). Here we examine how real-time planning processes in production differ for unaccusatives and unergatives. We build on previous studies on lookahead effects in sentence planning that show that verbs are planned before a deep object is uttered but not before a deep subject is uttered (Momma, Slevc, and Phillips 2015, 2016). (We use terms like deep subject in a theory-neutral fashion, with no intended commitment to a specific syntactic encoding.) This line of research sheds light on the broader issue of how the theory of argument structure relates to sentence production.

[1]  David M. Perlmutter Impersonal Passives and the Unaccusative Hypothesis , 1978 .

[2]  D. Swinney,et al.  The Leaf Fell (the Leaf): The Online Processing of Unaccusatives , 2008, Linguistic Inquiry.

[3]  Hagit Borer,et al.  Syntactic Cliticization and Lexical Cliticization: The Case of Hebrew Dative Clitics , 1986 .

[4]  Miseon Lee,et al.  Agrammatic aphasic production and comprehension of unaccusative verbs in sentence contexts , 2004, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[5]  Luigi Rizzi,et al.  Monkey morpho-syntax and merge-based systems , 2016 .

[6]  T. Landauer,et al.  A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .

[7]  Cynthia K Thompson,et al.  Unaccusative verb production in agrammatic aphasia: the argument structure complexity hypothesis , 2003, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[8]  David Caplan,et al.  Production and comprehension of unaccusatives in aphasia , 2009 .

[9]  益子 真由美 Argument Structure , 1993, The Lexicon.

[10]  Kathryn Bock,et al.  Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .

[11]  Holly P. Branigan,et al.  Contributions of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during production , 2008 .

[12]  A. Roelofs,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.

[13]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[14]  V. Ferreira,et al.  The Oxford Handbook of Language Production , 2014 .

[15]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Lectures on Government and Binding , 1981 .

[16]  L. Gleitman,et al.  On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. , 2007, Journal of memory and language.

[17]  A. Belletti,et al.  Psych-verbs and θ-theory , 1988 .

[18]  A. Meyer Lexical Access in Phrase and Sentence Production: Results from Picture–Word Interference Experiments , 1996 .

[19]  J. Kegl,et al.  Levels of Representation and Units of Access Relevant to Agrammatism , 1995, Brain and Language.

[20]  H. Schriefers,et al.  Producing Simple Sentences: Results from Picture–Word Interference Experiments , 1998 .

[21]  Philippe Schlenker,et al.  Monkey semantics: two ‘dialects’ of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls , 2014 .

[22]  C. Thompson,et al.  Real-time production of unergative and unaccusative sentences in normal and agrammatic speakers: An eyetracking study , 2011, Aphasiology.

[23]  Johan Rooryck,et al.  Phrase structure and the lexicon , 1996 .

[24]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Message formulation and structural assembly: Describing ''easy'' and ''hard'' events with preferred and dispreferred syntactic structures , 2014 .

[25]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Campbell's monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Thomas G. Bever,et al.  Empty categories access their antecedents during comprehension: unaccusatives in Spanish: unaccusatives in Spanish , 1997 .

[27]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  Liliane Haegeman,et al.  Introduction to Government and Binding Theory , 1991 .

[29]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Handbook of Psycholinguistics , 2011 .

[30]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[31]  B. Levin Unaccusativity: At the Syntax-Lexical Semantics Interface , 1994 .

[32]  Christina S. Kim Structural and Thematic Information in Sentence Production , 2006 .

[33]  A. Kratzer Severing the External Argument from its Verb , 1996 .

[34]  Colin Phillips,et al.  The timing of verb selection in Japanese sentence production. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  Tobias Riede,et al.  The relationship between acoustic structure and semantic information in Diana monkey alarm vocalization. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[36]  Gerard Kempen,et al.  The lexicalization process in sentence production and naming: indirect election of words , 1983, Cognition.

[37]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Campbell's Monkeys Use Affixation to Alter Call Meaning , 2009, PloS one.

[38]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .

[39]  W. Quine On the Reasons for Indeterminacy of Translation , 1970 .