Mechanisms for accessing lexical representations for output: Evidence from a category-specific semantic deficit

We report the performance of a neurologically impaired patient, JJ, whose oral reading of words exceeded his naming and comprehension performance for the same words--a pattern of performance that has been previously presented as evidence for "direct, nonsemantic, lexical" routes to output in reading. However, detailed analyses of JJ's reading and comprehension revealed two results that do not follow directly from the "direct route" hypothesis: (1) He accurately read aloud all orthophonologically regular words and just those irregular words for which he demonstrated some comprehension (as indicated by correct responses or within-category semantic errors in naming and comprehension tasks); and (2) his reading errors on words that were not comprehended at all (but were recognized as words) were phonologically plausible (e.g., soot read as "suit"). We account for these results by proposing that preserved sublexical mechanisms for converting print to sound, together with partially preserved semantic information, serve to mediate the activation of representations in the phonological output lexicon in the task of reading aloud. We present similar arguments for postulating an interaction between sublexical mechanisms and lexical output components of the spelling process.

[1]  E Capitani,et al.  Progressive language impairment without dementia: a case with isolated category specific semantic defect. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[2]  J. E. Stone,et al.  Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT–R) , 1989, Diagnostique.

[3]  James A. Reggia,et al.  Empirically derived probabilities for grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences in english , 1987 .

[4]  M. Coltheart Lexical access in simple reading tasks , 1978 .

[5]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Aspects of the Spelling Process: Evidence from a Case of Acquired Dysgraphia , 1986 .

[6]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The multiple semantics hypothesis: Multiple confusions? , 1990 .

[7]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Wernicke's aphasia and normal language processing: A case study in cognitive neuropsychology , 1983, Cognition.

[8]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Selective impairment of semantics in lexical processing , 1990 .

[9]  M Coltheart,et al.  Bilingual Biscriptal Deep Dyslexia , 1984, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[10]  E. Renzi,et al.  Normative Data and Screening Power of a Shortened Version of the Token Test , 1978, Cortex.

[11]  D. Neary,et al.  Deep Dyslexia , 1986 .

[12]  Richard J. Brown Neuropsychology Mental Structure , 1989 .

[13]  T. Shallice,et al.  Category specific semantic impairments , 1984 .

[14]  K. Patterson Lexical but nonsemantic spelling , 1986 .

[15]  J. Raven Coloured progressive matrices : sets A, Ab, B , 1956 .

[16]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  On telling your fruit from your vegetables: a consideration of category-specific deficits after brain damage , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  E. Warrington,et al.  Transcoding Sound to Spelling: Single or Multiple Sound Unit Correspondence? , 1987, Cortex.

[18]  E. Warrington,et al.  Phonological Reading: Phenomena and Paradoxes , 1986, Cortex.

[19]  E. Warrington,et al.  Category specific access dysphasia. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Surface dyslexia. , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[21]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition , 1984 .

[22]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The Interaction of Lexical and Non-Lexical Processing Mechanisms: Evidence from Anomia , 1991, Cortex.

[23]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Category-specific naming and comprehension impairment: a double dissociation. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Where Do Semantic Errors Come From? , 1990, Cortex.

[25]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[26]  Daniel B. Hier,et al.  Incongruous oral and written naming Evidence for a subdivision of the syndrome of Wernicke's aphasia , 1977, Brain and Language.

[27]  R. Goodman-Schulman Orthographic Ambiguity: Comments on Baxter and Warrington , 1988, Cortex.

[28]  W. Haenszel,et al.  Statistical aspects of the analysis of data from retrospective studies of disease. , 1959, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[29]  Rita Sloan Berndt,et al.  Category-specific deficits in aphasia , 1988 .

[30]  E. Funnell Phonological processes in reading: new evidence from acquired dyslexia. , 1983 .

[31]  David R. Beukelman,et al.  Clinical Management of Dysarthric Speakers , 1987 .

[32]  R. Berndt,et al.  Category-specific naming deficit following cerebral infarction , 1985, Nature.

[33]  David N. Levine,et al.  Language in the absence of inner speech , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  S. Black,et al.  Semantic encoding of pictures and words: Some neuropsychological observations , 1988 .

[35]  K. Heilman,et al.  Linguistic semantic agraphia: A dissociation of the lexical spelling system from semantics , 1986, Brain and Language.

[36]  E. Warrington,et al.  Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  D. Margolin Cognitive neuropsychology in clinical practice , 1992 .