How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief

Forty-four children (mean 3.8 years) were given three false belief, a working memory, and four language tasks (each designed to tap a different aspect of syntax or semantics), and were tested again 6 months later. Once the range of scores in the language and false belief tasks were equated, there was a bidirectional relation between language and theory of mind. There was no evidence for syntax playing a unique role in the contribution of language to theory of mind. No one measure of syntax or semantics was more likely than any other to predict later false belief. Nor was false belief related more to one aspect of later language (syntax vs. semantics) than another. Our data, taken with other findings, are consistent with the idea that both syntax and semantics contribute to false belief understanding. Working memory did not mediate the relation between language and theory of mind, nor did it facilitate later false belief.

[1]  Josef Perner,et al.  Metalinguistic awareness and theory of mind: Just two words for the same thing? , 1998 .

[2]  M. Siegal,et al.  Where to look first for children's knowledge of false beliefs , 1991, Cognition.

[3]  Jennie E. Pyers,et al.  Complements to cognition: a longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding , 2002 .

[4]  M. J. Farrar,et al.  Early language development and the emergence of a theory of mind∗ , 2002 .

[5]  E. Gibson Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies , 1998, Cognition.

[6]  Josef Perner,et al.  Want that is understood well before say that, think that, and false belief: a test of de Villiers's linguistic determinism on German-speaking children. , 2003, Child development.

[7]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Links between Social Understanding and Early Word Learning: Challenges to Current Accounts. , 2001 .

[8]  Kimberly A. Townsend A study of comparisons between the Joliet three minute speech and language screen and the Test of early language development (TELD) , 1985 .

[9]  Stephanie M. Carlson,et al.  How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory , 2002 .

[10]  David R. Olson,et al.  On the origins of beliefs and other intentional states in children. , 1988 .

[11]  Michael Siegal,et al.  Evidence for cognition without grammar from causal reasoning and ‘theory of mind’ in an agrammatic aphasic patient , 2000, Current Biology.

[12]  P. Bloom Précis of How Children Learn the Meanings of Words , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  M. Chandler,et al.  Small-scale deceit: deception as a marker of two-, three-, and four-year-olds' early theories of mind. , 1989, Child development.

[14]  T. Ruffman,et al.  The relation between children's and mothers' mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. , 2002, Child development.

[15]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The phonological loop as a language learning device. , 1998, Psychological review.

[16]  J. Astington,et al.  A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[17]  David R. Olson,et al.  Working memory and children's developing understanding of mind , 1998 .

[18]  J. Villiers,et al.  Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs. , 2000 .

[19]  J. Astington,et al.  Cognitive Factors and Family Structure Associated with Theory of Mind Development in Young Children. , 1996 .

[20]  Finding your marbles: does preschoolers' strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind? , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[21]  U. Frith Autism and theory of mind in everyday life , 1994 .

[22]  C. Lewis,et al.  Three-year-olds' problems with false belief: conceptual deficit or linguistic artifact? , 1990, Child development.

[23]  J. Perner Understanding the Representational Mind , 1993 .

[24]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Longitudinal Relations Between 2-Year-Olds' Language and 4-Year-Olds' Theory of Mind , 2001 .

[25]  D R Olson,et al.  The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[26]  B. Scholl,et al.  Minds, modules, and meta-analysis. , 2001, Child development.

[27]  J. Dunn,et al.  Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: individual differences and interrelations. , 1999, Child development.

[28]  C. Nixon,et al.  Social interaction skills and theory of mind in young children. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[29]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: links between preschoolers' theory of mind and semantic development. , 2001, Child development.

[30]  W. Clements,et al.  Promoting the Transition from Implicit to Explicit Understanding: A Training Study of False Belief , 2000 .

[31]  Helen Tager-Flusberg,et al.  The influence of language on theory of mind: a training study. , 2003, Developmental science.

[32]  J. Dunn,et al.  “Pretend you didn't know”: Preschoolers' talk about mental states in pretend play ☆ , 1997 .

[33]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[34]  L. Youngblade,et al.  Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: individual differences and their antecedents. , 1991, Child development.

[35]  T. Ruffman,et al.  Social understanding in autism: eye gaze as a measure of core insights. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[36]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[37]  P. Bloom,et al.  Preschoolers are sensitive to the speaker's knowledge when learning proper names. , 2002, Child development.

[38]  Charlie Lewis,et al.  Children's Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development , 1994 .

[39]  Marjorie Taylor,et al.  The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. , 1997, Child development.

[40]  F. Happé The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. , 1995, Child development.

[41]  Josef Perner,et al.  Theory of mind and self-control: more than a common problem of inhibition. , 2002, Child development.

[42]  J. Astington,et al.  The future of theory-of-mind research: understanding motivational states, the role of language, and real-world consequences. , 2001, Child development.

[43]  D. Furrow,et al.  Mental terms in mothers' and children's speech: similarities and relationships , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[44]  P. Carruthers The cognitive functions of language , 2002, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[45]  Ian Apperly,et al.  False belief reasoning and the acquisition of relative clause sentences. , 2003, Child development.

[46]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia , 1976, Brain and Language.

[47]  Thomas R. Keenan Memory Span as a Predictor of False Belief Understanding , 1998 .

[48]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Against the Cartesian view on mind: Young children's difficulty with own false beliefs , 1991 .

[49]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[50]  P. Bloom,et al.  Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind , 2000, Cognition.

[51]  Claire Hughes,et al.  Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability , 1998 .

[52]  H. Wimmer,et al.  False Belief Understanding in Young Children: Explanations do not Develop Before Predictions , 1998 .

[53]  W L Custer,et al.  A comparison of young children's understanding of contradictory representations in pretense, memory, and belief. , 1996, Child development.

[54]  Alan Garnham,et al.  How language relates to belief, desire, and emotion understanding , 2003 .

[55]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit , 1987 .

[56]  Lori K. Keith Book Review: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals - Preschool , 1994 .

[57]  Chris Pratt,et al.  The development of children's theory of mind: The working memory explanation , 1995 .

[58]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[59]  Claire Hughes,et al.  Finding your marbles: does preschoolers' strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind? , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[60]  S. Gathercole,et al.  Phonological Working Memory and Spoken Language Development in Young Children , 1996 .

[61]  Michael Siegal,et al.  Language and thought: the fundamental significance of conversational awareness for cognitive development , 1999 .

[62]  M. Doherty,et al.  Children's understanding of homonymy: metalinguistic awareness and false belief , 2000, Journal of Child Language.