Technology based evolution? A biometric test of the effects of handsize versus tool form on efficiency in an experimental cutting task

The use of stone cutting tools opened a novel adaptive niche for hominins. Hence, it has been hypothesised that biomechanical adaptations evolved to maximise efficiency when using such tools. Here, we test experimentally whether biometric variation influences the efficiency of simple cutting tools (n = 60 participants). Grip strength and handsize were measured in each participant. 30 participants used flint flakes, while the other 30 used small (unhafted) steel blades. Variations in basic parameters of tool form (length, width, thickness, cutting edge length) were recorded for the 30 flint flakes. It was ensured that mean handsize and strength in each participant group were not significantly different to investigate the effect of tool variation. The experimental task required cutting through a 10 mm-diameter hessian rope. Cutting efficiency was measured using both ‘Number of cutting strokes required’ and ‘Total time taken’. Results show that both efficiency measures were significantly correlated with handsize using all 60 participants. However, no significant differences were found between the flake and blade groups in terms of mean efficiency. Nor was any significant relationship found between tool form parameters and efficiency in the flint flake group. We stress that our results do not imply that tool form has no impact on tool efficiency, but rather that – all other things being equal – biometric variation has a statistically significant influence on efficiency variation when using simple cutting tools. These results demonstrate that biomechanical parameters related directly to efficiency of use, may plausibly have been subject to selection in the earliest stone tool-using hominins.

[1]  M W Marzke,et al.  Comparative 3D quantitative analyses of trapeziometacarpal joint surface curvatures among living catarrhines and fossil hominins. , 2009, American journal of physical anthropology.

[2]  Henry T. Bunn,et al.  Archaeological evidence for meat-eating by Plio-Pleistocene hominids from Koobi Fora and Olduvai Gorge , 1981, Nature.

[3]  Payson Sheets,et al.  Time, Energy, and Stone Tools , 1990 .

[4]  R. L. Susman Hand function and tool behavior in early hominids. , 1998, Journal of human evolution.

[5]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Primate Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior , 2001, Springer Japan.

[6]  George C. Frison,et al.  Experimental Use of Clovis Weaponry and Tools on African Elephants , 1989, American Antiquity.

[7]  R. Jeske Energetic Efficiency and Lithic Technology: An Upper Mississippian Example , 1992, American Antiquity.

[8]  S. Ambrose Paleolithic Technology and Human Evolution , 2001, Science.

[9]  Lawrence H. Keeley,et al.  Experimental Determination of Stone Tool Uses: A Microwear Analysis , 1979 .

[10]  R. L. Linscheid,et al.  EMG study of hand muscle recruitment during hard hammer percussion manufacture of Oldowan tools. , 1998, American journal of physical anthropology.

[11]  O. J. Lewis,et al.  Functional morphology of the evolving hand and foot , 1990 .

[12]  M. Domínguez‐Rodrigo,et al.  Configurational approach to identifying the earliest hominin butchers , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  S. Washburn Speculations on the interrelations of the history of tools and biological evolution. , 1959, Human biology.

[14]  R. L. Susman,et al.  Fossil evidence for early hominid tool use. , 1994, Science.

[15]  O O Okunribido,et al.  A survey of hand anthropometry of female rural farm workers in Ibadan, Western Nigeria , 2000, Ergonomics.

[16]  G. Yamakoshi Ecology of Tool Use in Wild Chimpanzees: Toward Reconstruction of Early Hominid Evolution , 2008 .

[17]  C. Marean,et al.  Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia , 2010, Nature.

[18]  D E Crabtree,et al.  Experimental Manufacture of Wooden Implements with Tools of Flaked Stone , 1968, Science.

[19]  S. Shultz,et al.  Digit ratios predict polygyny in early apes, Ardipithecus, Neanderthals and early modern humans but not in Australopithecus , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  Michael J. Rogers,et al.  2.6-Million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. , 2003, Journal of human evolution.

[21]  Jatmiko,et al.  The Primitive Wrist of Homo floresiensis and Its Implications for Hominin Evolution , 2007, Science.

[22]  Matthew W. Tocheri,et al.  The evolutionary history of the hominin hand since the last common ancestor of Pan and Homo , 2008, Journal of anatomy.

[23]  M W Marzke,et al.  Precision grips, hand morphology, and tools. , 1997, American journal of physical anthropology.

[24]  Mary W. Marzke,et al.  Chimpanzee and human grips: A new classification with a focus on evolutionary morphology , 1996, International Journal of Primatology.

[25]  Peter S. Ungar,et al.  The evolution of human diet: The known, the unknown, and the unknowable , 2004 .

[26]  R. L. Susman Hand of Paranthropus robustus from Member 1, Swartkrans: fossil evidence for tool behavior. , 1988, Science.

[27]  T. Pickering,et al.  Cutmarks and hominid handedness , 2008 .

[28]  Zachary J. Davis,et al.  Experimental Tests of Middle Palaeolithic Spear Points Using a Calibrated Crossbow , 2001 .

[29]  N. Toth,et al.  The cutting edge : new approaches to the archaeology of human origins , 2009 .

[30]  A. Clerke,et al.  Effects of hand shape on maximal isometric grip strength and its reliability in teenagers. , 2005, Journal of hand therapy : official journal of the American Society of Hand Therapists.

[31]  E. Lonsdorf What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[32]  R. L. Susman,et al.  Functional and morphological affinities of the subadult hand (O.H. 7) from Olduvai Gorge. , 1979, American journal of physical anthropology.

[33]  Blandine Bril,et al.  Stone knapping : the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour , 2005 .

[34]  H. Roche,et al.  Early hominid stone tool production and technical skill 2.34 Myr ago in West Turkana, Kenya , 1999, Nature.

[35]  J. Napier The prehensile movements of the human hand. , 1956, The Journal of bone and joint surgery. British volume.

[36]  M. Ghiselin,et al.  Coevolution: Genes, Culture, and Human Diversity , 1991, Politics and the Life Sciences.

[37]  M W Marzke,et al.  Functional capabilities of modern and fossil hominid hands: three-dimensional analysis of trapezia. , 2003, American journal of physical anthropology.

[38]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans , 2005, Nature.

[39]  Metin I. Eren,et al.  Middle Paleolithic Skill Level and the Individual Knapper: An Experiment , 2011, American Antiquity.

[40]  R. Sevcik,et al.  Pan the Tool-Maker: Investigations into the Stone Tool-Making and Tool-Using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1993 .

[41]  S. Lycett,et al.  Are behavioral differences among wild chimpanzee communities genetic or cultural? An assessment using tool-use data and phylogenetic methods. , 2010, American journal of physical anthropology.

[42]  J. Landsmeer Power Grip and Precision Handling , 1962, Annals of the rheumatic diseases.

[43]  A. Whiten,et al.  Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  L. Aiello,et al.  The Expensive-Tissue Hypothesis: The Brain and the Digestive System in Human and Primate Evolution , 1995, Current Anthropology.

[45]  David L. Clarke,et al.  Models in archaeology , 1972 .

[46]  M. Orne On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. , 1962 .

[47]  Michael J. Rogers,et al.  Cutmarked bones from Pliocene archaeological sites at Gona, Afar, Ethiopia: implications for the function of the world's oldest stone tools. , 2005, Journal of human evolution.

[48]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  Julie C. Stout,et al.  Stone Tool-Making and Brain Activation: Position Emission Tomography (PET) Studies , 2000 .

[50]  W. McGrew Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution , 1992 .

[51]  M. Marzke,et al.  Evolution of the human hand: approaches to acquiring, analysing and interpreting the anatomical evidence , 2000, Journal of anatomy.

[52]  T. Plummer Flaked stones and old bones: biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. , 2004, American journal of physical anthropology.

[53]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  Analytical Approaches to Palaeolithic Technologies: An Introduction , 2010 .

[54]  R. Potts,et al.  Cutmarks made by stone tools on bones from Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania , 1981, Nature.

[55]  V. Mathiowetz,et al.  Grip and pinch strength: normative data for adults. , 1985, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[56]  M. Marzke,et al.  Hominid hand use in the pliocene and pleistocene: Evidence from experimental archaeology and comparative morphology* , 1986 .

[57]  Sophie Collins,et al.  Experimental investigations into edge performance and its implications for stone artefact reduction modelling , 2008 .

[58]  Hansjürgen Müller-Beck,et al.  A History of Flint-Knapping Experimentation, 1838-1976 [and Comments and Reply] , 1978, Current Anthropology.

[59]  M. Feldman,et al.  Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. , 1996, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[60]  C. Clarkson An Index of Invasiveness for the Measurement of Unifacial and Bifacial Retouch: A Theoretical, Experimental and Archaeological Verification , 2002 .

[61]  Phillip L. Walker,et al.  Butchering and Stone Tool Function , 1978, American Antiquity.

[62]  W A Niewoehner,et al.  Behavioral inferences from the Skhul/Qafzeh early modern human hand remains , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[63]  Gary T. Garufi,et al.  Continuing Investigations into the Stone Tool-making and Tool-using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1999 .

[64]  Bernard Wood,et al.  Older than the Oldowan? Rethinking the emergence of hominin tool use , 2003 .

[65]  Alan Walker,et al.  The costs of becoming a predator , 1989 .

[66]  S. Lycett,et al.  Cladistic analyses of behavioural variation in wild Pan troglodytes: exploring the chimpanzee culture hypothesis. , 2009, Journal of human evolution.

[67]  Peter Jones Experimental butchery with modern stone tools and its relevance for Palaeolithic archaeology , 1980 .

[68]  K. Laland,et al.  How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together , 2010, Nature Reviews Genetics.

[69]  Anne Delagnes,et al.  Late Pliocene hominid knapping skills: the case of Lokalalei 2C, West Turkana, Kenya. , 2005, Journal of human evolution.

[70]  Steven Mithen,et al.  Why are some handaxes symmetrical? Testing the influence of handaxe morphology on butchery effectiveness , 2007 .

[71]  Andrew P. Bradbury,et al.  Flake Size from Platform Attributes: Predictive and Empirical Approaches , 2000 .

[72]  Brian G Richmond,et al.  Early hominin diet included diverse terrestrial and aquatic animals 1.95 Ma in East Turkana, Kenya , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[73]  C. Darwin The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex: INDEX , 1871 .

[74]  Thomas Reilly Advances in Sport, Leisure and Ergonomics , 2003 .

[75]  Charlotte D. Pevny,et al.  A biface and blade core efficiency experiment: implications for Early Paleoindian technological organization , 2010 .

[76]  M. Feldman,et al.  Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. , 1981, Monographs in population biology.

[77]  T. Steele A unique hominin menu dated to 1.95 million years ago , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[78]  T. Matsuzawa,et al.  Social influences on ant-dipping acquisition in the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[79]  M. Eren,et al.  Are Upper Paleolithic blade cores more productive than Middle Paleolithic discoidal cores? A replication experiment. , 2008, Journal of human evolution.

[80]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  The evolution and cultural transmission of percussive technology: integrating evidence from palaeoanthropology and primatology. , 2009, Journal of human evolution.

[81]  J. Crow The genetic basis of evolutionary change , 1975 .

[82]  Michelle Y. Merrill,et al.  The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. , 1999, Journal of human evolution.

[83]  Kathryn Weedman Arthur,et al.  Feminine Knowledge and Skill Reconsidered: Women and Flaked Stone Tools , 2010 .

[84]  Curtis W. Marean,et al.  Meat eating and human evolution , 2002 .

[85]  P. Richerson,et al.  Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. , 2004 .

[86]  Nerissa Russell,et al.  Relative frequency of butchering cutmarks produced by obsidian and flint: an experimental approach , 2007 .

[87]  W. Straus,,et al.  Rudimentary Digits in Primates , 1942, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[88]  J. Spuhler The Evolution of Man's Capacity for Culture , 1961 .

[89]  J. Napier,et al.  Fossil Hand Bones from Olduvai Gorge , 1962, Nature.

[90]  D. Schmitt,et al.  EMG of the human flexor pollicis longus muscle: implications for the evolution of hominid tool use. , 1998, Journal of human evolution.

[91]  A. Whiten,et al.  Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees , 2005, Nature.

[92]  S. Moyà-Solà,et al.  Morphological affinities of the Australopithecus afarensis hand on the basis of manual proportions and relative thumb length. , 2003, Journal of human evolution.

[93]  Mary M. Prasciunas Bifacial Cores and Flake Production Efficiency: An Experimental Test of Technological Assumptions , 2007, American Antiquity.

[94]  R. Shaw,et al.  Ulu Knife Use in Western Alaska: A Comparative Ethnoarchaeological Study1 , 2003, Current Anthropology.

[95]  C. Clarkson,et al.  Experimental evaluation of Kuhn's geometric index of reduction and the flat-flake problem , 2005 .

[96]  R. L. Susman,et al.  Who Made the Oldowan Tools? Fossil Evidence for Tool Behavior in Plio-Pleistocene Hominids , 1991, Journal of Anthropological Research.

[97]  D. Biro,et al.  Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments , 2003, Animal Cognition.

[98]  E. Sarmiento Anatomy of the hominoid wrist joint: Its evolutionary and functional implications , 1988, International Journal of Primatology.

[99]  A M Nevill,et al.  Modelling handgrip strength in the presence of confounding variables: results from the Allied Dunbar National Fitness Survey , 2000, Ergonomics.

[100]  S. Semaw,et al.  The World's Oldest Stone Artefacts from Gona, Ethiopia: Their Implications for Understanding Stone Technology and Patterns of Human Evolution Between 2·6-1·5 Million Years Ago , 2000 .

[101]  D. L. Clarke Models and paradigms in contemporary Archaeology , 1972 .

[102]  G. Isaac,et al.  The diet of early man: aspects of archaeological evidence from Lower and Middle Pleistocene Sites in Africa. , 1971, World archaeology.