The Big Man Mechanism: how prestige fosters cooperation and creates prosocial leaders

Anthropological evidence from diverse societies suggests that prestige-based leadership may provide a foundation for cooperation in many contexts. Here, inspired by such ethnographic observations and building on a foundation of existing research on the evolution of prestige, we develop a set of formal models to explore when an evolved prestige psychology might drive the cultural evolution of n-person cooperation, and how such a cultural evolutionary process might create novel selection pressures for genes that make prestigious individuals more prosocial. Our results reveal (i) how prestige can foster the cultural emergence of cooperation by generating correlated behavioural phenotypes, both between leaders and followers, and among followers; (ii) why, in the wake of cultural evolution, natural selection favours genes that make prestigious leaders more prosocial, but only when groups are relatively small; and (iii), why the effectiveness of status differences in generating cooperation in large groups depends on cultural transmission (and not primarily on deference or coercion). Our theoretical framework, and the specific predictions made by these models, sketch out an interdisciplinary research programme that cross-cuts anthropology, biology, psychology and economics. Some of our predictions find support from laboratory work in behavioural economics and are consistent with several real-world patterns.

[1]  A. Kroeber Handbook of the Indians of California , 1925 .

[2]  R. R. Blake,et al.  Volunteering as a function of field structure. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[3]  M. Rosenbaum The effect of stimulus and background factors on the volunteering response. , 1956, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[4]  Marshall Sahlins,et al.  Poor Man, Rich Man, Big-man, Chief: Political Types in Melanesia and Polynesia , 1963, Comparative Studies in Society and History.

[5]  M. Test,et al.  Models and helping: naturalistic studies in aiding behavior. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  D. Maybury-Lewis Akwẽ-Shavante society , 1967 .

[7]  Irving Goldman,et al.  Ancient Polynesian society , 1972 .

[8]  R. Paine Animals as Capital: Comparisons among Northern Nomadic Herders and Hunters , 1971 .

[9]  W. Hamilton Innate social aptitudes of man: an approach from evolutionary genetics , 1975 .

[10]  J. P. Rushton,et al.  Modeling, vicarious reinforcement and extraversion on blood donating in adults: Immediate and long‐term effects , 1977 .

[11]  Peter J. Richerson,et al.  A dual inheritance model of the human evolutionary process I: Basic postulates and a simple model , 1978 .

[12]  William C. Sturtevant,et al.  Handbook of North American Indians , 1978 .

[13]  C. Lumsden Culture and the Evolutionary Process, Robert Boyd, Peter J. Richerson. University of Chicago Press, Chicago & London (1985), viii, +301. Price $29.95 , 1986 .

[14]  M. Godelier,et al.  The Making of Great Men. Male domination and power among the New Guinea Baruya , 1987 .

[15]  Allen Johnson,et al.  The Evolution of Human Societies: From Foraging Group to Agrarian State , 1988 .

[16]  Allen Johnson,et al.  The evolution of human societies : from foraging group to agrarian state , 1988 .

[17]  B. Mullen,et al.  Jaywalking as a Function of Model Behavior , 1990 .

[18]  L. Lindstrom BIG MEN AS ANCESTORS: INSPIRATION AND COPYRIGHTS ON TANNA (VANUATU) , 1990 .

[19]  P. Allison Cultural relatedness under oblique and horizontal transmission rules , 1992 .

[20]  D. Queller,et al.  A GENERAL MODEL FOR KIN SELECTION , 1992, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[21]  Paul A. Bell,et al.  Conformity as a Determinant of Behavior in a Resource Dilemma , 1994 .

[22]  Peter J. Richerson,et al.  Why does culture increase human adaptability , 1995 .

[23]  P. Sillitoe An Introduction to the Anthropology of Melanesia: Culture and Tradition , 1998 .

[24]  B. Brenton Food and the Status Quest: An Interdisciplinary Perspective , 1998 .

[25]  Sita Venkateswar,et al.  THE ANDAMAN ISLANDERS , 1999 .

[26]  Jorgen Wit,et al.  Social Learning in a Common Interest Voting Game , 1999 .

[27]  E. Smith,et al.  Turtle hunting and tombstone opening. public generosity as costly signaling. , 2000, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[28]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[29]  H. Gintis,et al.  Costly signaling and cooperation. , 2001, Journal of theoretical biology.

[30]  L. Vesterlund,et al.  After You - Endogenous Sequencing in Voluntary Contribution Games , 2005 .

[31]  Jeffrey P. Carpenter,et al.  When in Rome: conformity and the provision of public goods , 2004 .

[32]  K. Laland Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[33]  J. Henrich Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation , 2004 .

[34]  W. Güth,et al.  Leading by example with and without exclusion power in voluntary contribution experiments , 2007 .

[35]  Martin H. Levinson Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution , 2006 .

[36]  V. Jaswal,et al.  Turning Believers into Skeptics: 3-Year-Olds' Sensitivity to Cues to Speaker Credibility , 2007 .

[37]  L. Kirkpatrick,et al.  Dominance and prestige as differential predictors of aggression and testosterone levels in men , 2007 .

[38]  Martin Sefton,et al.  Leading-by-example and signaling in voluntary contribution games: an experimental study , 2007 .

[39]  J. Henrich,et al.  Why Humans Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation , 2007 .

[40]  Laurent Lehmann,et al.  Cultural Transmission Can Inhibit the Evolution of Altruistic Helping , 2008, The American Naturalist.

[41]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Constraining free riding in public goods games: designated solitary punishers can sustain human cooperation , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  Michael Doebeli,et al.  A simple and general explanation for the evolution of altruism , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[43]  M. Gurven,et al.  The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society. , 2008, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[44]  P. Bloom,et al.  Three- and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning , 2008, Cognition.

[45]  Dominic D. P. Johnson,et al.  The Origins and Evolution of Leadership , 2009, Current Biology.

[46]  Gunilla Stenberg,et al.  Selectivity in Infant Social Referencing. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[47]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  How cultural evolutionary theory can inform social psychology and vice versa. , 2009, Psychological review.

[48]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution , 2009 .

[49]  K. Laland,et al.  How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together , 2010, Nature Reviews Genetics.

[50]  Michael Gurven,et al.  Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to dominance and prestige , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[51]  Paul L. Hooper,et al.  A theory of leadership in human cooperative groups. , 2010, Journal of theoretical biology.

[52]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status , 2010 .

[53]  Nicholas A. Christakis,et al.  Cooperative behavior cascades in human social networks , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[54]  S. Birch,et al.  Two-year-olds are vigilant of others' non-verbal cues to credibility. , 2010, Developmental science.

[55]  Enrique Fatas,et al.  Cooperation and Status in Organizations , 2010 .

[56]  L. Vesterlund,et al.  The Effect of Status on Charitable Giving , 2010 .

[57]  J. Henrich,et al.  The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[58]  J. Henrich,et al.  Culture-gene coevolutionary theory and children’s selective social learning , 2011 .

[59]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  On the nature of cultural transmission networks: evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[60]  Kathleen H Corriveau,et al.  Young children's selective trust in informants , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[61]  K. Kwak,et al.  Uncertainty matters: impact of stimulus ambiguity on infant social referencing , 2011 .

[62]  M. J. O’Brien,et al.  Adult Learners in a Novel Environment Use Prestige-Biased Social Learning , 2012, Evolutionary psychology : an international journal of evolutionary approaches to psychology and behavior.

[63]  R. Boyd,et al.  A Bayesian approach to the evolution of social learning , 2012 .

[64]  S. Gächter,et al.  Who Makes a Good Leader? Cooperativeness, Optimism, and Leading‐By‐Example , 2012 .

[65]  M. Gurven,et al.  When the strong punish: Why net costs of punishment are often negligible , 2012, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[66]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander's differential attention to potential models influences children's learning , 2012 .

[67]  E. Cartwright,et al.  Leadership by Example in the Weak‐Link Game , 2013 .

[68]  Tom Foulsham,et al.  Two ways to the top: evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. , 2013, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[69]  Jessica L. Tracy,et al.  The Emotional Origins of a Social Learning Bias : Does the Pride Expression Cue Copying ? , 2012 .

[70]  Jessica L. Tracy,et al.  Cross-cultural evidence that the nonverbal expression of pride is an automatic status signal. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[71]  J. Henrich,et al.  Culture-Gene Coevolution, Large-Scale Cooperation, and the Shaping of Human Social Psychology , 2013 .

[72]  G. Lindeman,et al.  The Angelina Jolie effect , 2013, The Medical journal of Australia.

[73]  Diana M Eccles,et al.  The Angelina Jolie effect: how high celebrity profile can have a major impact on provision of cancer related services , 2014, Breast Cancer Research.

[74]  L. Marshall The !Kung of Nyae Nyae , 2014 .

[75]  M. Gurven,et al.  Leadership in an Egalitarian Society , 2014, Human nature.

[76]  Pat Barclay,et al.  Prosocial behavior and social status , 2014 .

[77]  C. Rueden The Roots and Fruits of Social Status in Small-Scale Human Societies , 2014 .

[78]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Transmission and development of costly punishment in children , 2015 .

[79]  Leadership in an egalitarian human society , 2015 .

[80]  J. Henrich The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter , 2015 .

[81]  Michael Muthukrishna,et al.  The when and who of social learning and conformist transmission , 2016 .

[82]  Katharina Burger,et al.  The Kung San Men Women And Work In A Foraging Society , 2016 .

[83]  J. Henrich,et al.  18. Cultural Evolution in Chimpanzees and Humans , 2017 .