The Big Man Mechanism: how prestige fosters cooperation and creates prosocial leaders
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Kroeber. Handbook of the Indians of California , 1925 .
[2] R. R. Blake,et al. Volunteering as a function of field structure. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[3] M. Rosenbaum. The effect of stimulus and background factors on the volunteering response. , 1956, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[4] Marshall Sahlins,et al. Poor Man, Rich Man, Big-man, Chief: Political Types in Melanesia and Polynesia , 1963, Comparative Studies in Society and History.
[5] M. Test,et al. Models and helping: naturalistic studies in aiding behavior. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[6] D. Maybury-Lewis. Akwẽ-Shavante society , 1967 .
[7] Irving Goldman,et al. Ancient Polynesian society , 1972 .
[8] R. Paine. Animals as Capital: Comparisons among Northern Nomadic Herders and Hunters , 1971 .
[9] W. Hamilton. Innate social aptitudes of man: an approach from evolutionary genetics , 1975 .
[10] J. P. Rushton,et al. Modeling, vicarious reinforcement and extraversion on blood donating in adults: Immediate and long‐term effects , 1977 .
[11] Peter J. Richerson,et al. A dual inheritance model of the human evolutionary process I: Basic postulates and a simple model , 1978 .
[12] William C. Sturtevant,et al. Handbook of North American Indians , 1978 .
[13] C. Lumsden. Culture and the Evolutionary Process, Robert Boyd, Peter J. Richerson. University of Chicago Press, Chicago & London (1985), viii, +301. Price $29.95 , 1986 .
[14] M. Godelier,et al. The Making of Great Men. Male domination and power among the New Guinea Baruya , 1987 .
[15] Allen Johnson,et al. The Evolution of Human Societies: From Foraging Group to Agrarian State , 1988 .
[16] Allen Johnson,et al. The evolution of human societies : from foraging group to agrarian state , 1988 .
[17] B. Mullen,et al. Jaywalking as a Function of Model Behavior , 1990 .
[18] L. Lindstrom. BIG MEN AS ANCESTORS: INSPIRATION AND COPYRIGHTS ON TANNA (VANUATU) , 1990 .
[19] P. Allison. Cultural relatedness under oblique and horizontal transmission rules , 1992 .
[20] D. Queller,et al. A GENERAL MODEL FOR KIN SELECTION , 1992, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[21] Paul A. Bell,et al. Conformity as a Determinant of Behavior in a Resource Dilemma , 1994 .
[22] Peter J. Richerson,et al. Why does culture increase human adaptability , 1995 .
[23] P. Sillitoe. An Introduction to the Anthropology of Melanesia: Culture and Tradition , 1998 .
[24] B. Brenton. Food and the Status Quest: An Interdisciplinary Perspective , 1998 .
[25] Sita Venkateswar,et al. THE ANDAMAN ISLANDERS , 1999 .
[26] Jorgen Wit,et al. Social Learning in a Common Interest Voting Game , 1999 .
[27] E. Smith,et al. Turtle hunting and tombstone opening. public generosity as costly signaling. , 2000, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.
[28] J. Henrich,et al. The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.
[29] H. Gintis,et al. Costly signaling and cooperation. , 2001, Journal of theoretical biology.
[30] L. Vesterlund,et al. After You - Endogenous Sequencing in Voluntary Contribution Games , 2005 .
[31] Jeffrey P. Carpenter,et al. When in Rome: conformity and the provision of public goods , 2004 .
[32] K. Laland. Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.
[33] J. Henrich. Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation , 2004 .
[34] W. Güth,et al. Leading by example with and without exclusion power in voluntary contribution experiments , 2007 .
[35] Martin H. Levinson. Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution , 2006 .
[36] V. Jaswal,et al. Turning Believers into Skeptics: 3-Year-Olds' Sensitivity to Cues to Speaker Credibility , 2007 .
[37] L. Kirkpatrick,et al. Dominance and prestige as differential predictors of aggression and testosterone levels in men , 2007 .
[38] Martin Sefton,et al. Leading-by-example and signaling in voluntary contribution games: an experimental study , 2007 .
[39] J. Henrich,et al. Why Humans Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation , 2007 .
[40] Laurent Lehmann,et al. Cultural Transmission Can Inhibit the Evolution of Altruistic Helping , 2008, The American Naturalist.
[41] Joseph Henrich,et al. Constraining free riding in public goods games: designated solitary punishers can sustain human cooperation , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[42] Michael Doebeli,et al. A simple and general explanation for the evolution of altruism , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[43] M. Gurven,et al. The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society. , 2008, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.
[44] P. Bloom,et al. Three- and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning , 2008, Cognition.
[45] Dominic D. P. Johnson,et al. The Origins and Evolution of Leadership , 2009, Current Biology.
[46] Gunilla Stenberg,et al. Selectivity in Infant Social Referencing. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[47] Alex Mesoudi,et al. How cultural evolutionary theory can inform social psychology and vice versa. , 2009, Psychological review.
[48] Joseph Henrich,et al. The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution , 2009 .
[49] K. Laland,et al. How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together , 2010, Nature Reviews Genetics.
[50] Michael Gurven,et al. Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to dominance and prestige , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[51] Paul L. Hooper,et al. A theory of leadership in human cooperative groups. , 2010, Journal of theoretical biology.
[52] Joseph Henrich,et al. Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status , 2010 .
[53] Nicholas A. Christakis,et al. Cooperative behavior cascades in human social networks , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[54] S. Birch,et al. Two-year-olds are vigilant of others' non-verbal cues to credibility. , 2010, Developmental science.
[55] Enrique Fatas,et al. Cooperation and Status in Organizations , 2010 .
[56] L. Vesterlund,et al. The Effect of Status on Charitable Giving , 2010 .
[57] J. Henrich,et al. The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[58] J. Henrich,et al. Culture-gene coevolutionary theory and children’s selective social learning , 2011 .
[59] Joseph Henrich,et al. On the nature of cultural transmission networks: evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[60] Kathleen H Corriveau,et al. Young children's selective trust in informants , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[61] K. Kwak,et al. Uncertainty matters: impact of stimulus ambiguity on infant social referencing , 2011 .
[62] M. J. O’Brien,et al. Adult Learners in a Novel Environment Use Prestige-Biased Social Learning , 2012, Evolutionary psychology : an international journal of evolutionary approaches to psychology and behavior.
[63] R. Boyd,et al. A Bayesian approach to the evolution of social learning , 2012 .
[64] S. Gächter,et al. Who Makes a Good Leader? Cooperativeness, Optimism, and Leading‐By‐Example , 2012 .
[65] M. Gurven,et al. When the strong punish: Why net costs of punishment are often negligible , 2012, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[66] Joseph Henrich,et al. Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander's differential attention to potential models influences children's learning , 2012 .
[67] E. Cartwright,et al. Leadership by Example in the Weak‐Link Game , 2013 .
[68] Tom Foulsham,et al. Two ways to the top: evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. , 2013, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[69] Jessica L. Tracy,et al. The Emotional Origins of a Social Learning Bias : Does the Pride Expression Cue Copying ? , 2012 .
[70] Jessica L. Tracy,et al. Cross-cultural evidence that the nonverbal expression of pride is an automatic status signal. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[71] J. Henrich,et al. Culture-Gene Coevolution, Large-Scale Cooperation, and the Shaping of Human Social Psychology , 2013 .
[72] G. Lindeman,et al. The Angelina Jolie effect , 2013, The Medical journal of Australia.
[73] Diana M Eccles,et al. The Angelina Jolie effect: how high celebrity profile can have a major impact on provision of cancer related services , 2014, Breast Cancer Research.
[74] L. Marshall. The !Kung of Nyae Nyae , 2014 .
[75] M. Gurven,et al. Leadership in an Egalitarian Society , 2014, Human nature.
[76] Pat Barclay,et al. Prosocial behavior and social status , 2014 .
[77] C. Rueden. The Roots and Fruits of Social Status in Small-Scale Human Societies , 2014 .
[78] Joseph Henrich,et al. Transmission and development of costly punishment in children , 2015 .
[79] Leadership in an egalitarian human society , 2015 .
[80] J. Henrich. The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter , 2015 .
[81] Michael Muthukrishna,et al. The when and who of social learning and conformist transmission , 2016 .
[82] Katharina Burger,et al. The Kung San Men Women And Work In A Foraging Society , 2016 .
[83] J. Henrich,et al. 18. Cultural Evolution in Chimpanzees and Humans , 2017 .