How Social Norms Promote Misleading Social Feedback and Inaccurate Self-Assessment

Self-assessments are often prone to error. Past research has identified cognitive and motivational biases that lead self-assessments astray. In the present paper, we discuss how behavior shaped by social norms leaves the negative information that people require for accurate self-assessments invisible. First, social norms lead people to suppress critical feedback in favor of more positive evaluations. Although people recognize that they prefer to provide positive feedback to others, they fail to consider that they might be the recipient of incomplete feedback. As a result, they are left with overconfident self-impressions. Second, social norms lead people to hide their own negative emotional experiences from others. Again, people are aware that this positivity norm influences their own behavior but do not apply this knowledge to their understanding of others. As a result, people regard their own negative emotions as more socially aberrant than is actually the case.

[1]  J. Gross,et al.  Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  M. Clark,et al.  "Walking on eggshells": how expressing relationship insecurities perpetuates them. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  S. Haves,et al.  Experiential Avoidance and Behavioral Disorders: A Functional Dimensional Approach to Diagnosis and Treatment , 2003 .

[4]  James J. Gross,et al.  Misery Has More Company Than People Think: Underestimating the Prevalence of Others’ Negative Emotions , 2011, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[5]  Kerri L. Johnson,et al.  Why the Unskilled are Unaware: Further Explorations of (Absent) Self-Insight Among the Incompetent , 2006, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[6]  L. Ross,et al.  Hedonic consequences of social comparison: a contrast of happy and unhappy people. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  P. Borkenau,et al.  Convergence of stranger ratings of personality and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured intelligence. , 1993 .

[8]  D. Wegner Ironic processes of mental control. , 1994, Psychological review.

[9]  G. Stricker American Psychological Association Code of Ethics , 2010 .

[10]  L. Ross,et al.  The overconfidence effect in social prediction. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  J. Penley,et al.  The Association of Coping to Physical and Psychological Health Outcomes: A Meta-Analytic Review , 2002, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[12]  J. Simpson,et al.  Emotion regulation in couples and families : pathways to dysfunction and health , 2006 .

[13]  D. Wegner,et al.  Thought suppression. , 2000, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  S. Hayes,et al.  Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An Experiential Approach to Behavior Change , 1999 .

[15]  Joseph P. Forgas,et al.  Emotion and Social Judgments , 2020 .

[16]  S. Orsillo,et al.  Acceptance and mindfulness-based approaches to anxiety: Conceptualization and treatment. , 2005 .

[17]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Feeling and facial efference: implications of the vascular theory of emotion. , 1989, Psychological review.

[18]  Tsukasa Nishida,et al.  Communication in personal relationships across cultures , 1996 .

[19]  C. Edgley,et al.  Information control in conversations: Honesty is not always the best policy , 1975 .

[20]  James A. Scepansky,et al.  Motivated sensitivity to preference-inconsistent information. , 1998 .

[21]  J. G. Miller Culture and development of everyday social explanation. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Constantine Sedikides,et al.  Accountability as a deterrent to self-enhancement: the search for mechanisms. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  E. Diener,et al.  Norms for experiencing emotions in different cultures: inter- and intranational differences. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  L. Ross The Intuitive Psychologist And His Shortcomings: Distortions in the Attribution Process1 , 1977 .

[25]  O. John,et al.  Emotion Regulation in Everyday Life. , 2006 .

[26]  David Dunning,et al.  How chronic self-views influence (and potentially mislead) estimates of performance. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  B. Depaulo,et al.  Truth and investment: lies are told to those who care. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  On the relative preponderance of empathic sorrow and its relation to commonsense morality , 2001 .

[29]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[30]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  Mood Congruity of Social Judgments , 1991 .

[31]  Penelope Brown,et al.  Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage , 1989 .

[32]  S. Srivastava,et al.  The social costs of emotional suppression: a prospective study of the transition to college. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  P. Ekman,et al.  The Repertoire of Nonverbal Behavior: Categories, Origins, Usage, and Coding , 1969 .

[34]  David Dunning,et al.  Self-Insight: Roadblocks and Detours on the Path to Knowing Thyself , 2005 .

[35]  Michael T. Motley,et al.  White lies in interpersonal communication: A taxonomy and preliminary investigation of social motivations , 1984 .

[36]  David G. Rand,et al.  the high Himalayas and Antarctica Phylogeography of microbial phototrophs in the dry valleys of Supplementary data tml , 2010 .

[37]  Ladd Wheeler,et al.  Social Comparison in Everyday Life , 1992 .

[38]  M. Morris,et al.  Culture and Cause: American and Chinese Attributions for Social and Physical Events , 1994 .

[39]  E. Goffman Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-To-Face Behavior , 1967 .

[40]  Daniel J. Wilson,et al.  Dark contrasts: The paradox of high rates of suicide in happy places , 2011 .

[41]  M. L. Klotz,et al.  Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. , 1995 .

[42]  D. Watson,et al.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Self-other Agreement in Personality and Affectivity: the Role of Acquaintanceship, Trait Visibility, and Assumed Similarity , 2022 .

[43]  Kerri L. Johnson,et al.  Why People Fail to Recognize Their Own Incompetence , 2003 .

[44]  Pino G. Audia,et al.  Self-Enhancement and Learning from Performance Feedback , 2012 .

[45]  Brandon E. Gibb,et al.  Co-rumination and Lifetime History of Depressive Disorders in Children , 2010, Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology : the official journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53.

[46]  Joan G. Miller Culture and the Development of Everyday Social Explanation , 1984 .

[47]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  O. John,et al.  Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  P. Borkenau,et al.  Convergence of stranger ratings of personality and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured intelligence. , 1993 .

[50]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Pluralistic ignorance: When similarity is interpreted as dissimilarity. , 1987 .

[51]  M. Alicke,et al.  Deconstructing the better-than-average effect. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[52]  E. Lemay,et al.  Caution: fragile! Regulating the interpersonal security of chronically insecure partners. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.