‘The Ostrich Problem’: motivated avoidance or rejection of information about goal progress

Monitoring one’s current standing with respect to goals can promote effective self-regulation. However, the present review suggests that there is an ostrich problem such that, in many instances, people have a tendency to “bury their head in the sand” and intentionally avoid or reject information that would help them to monitor their goal progress. For example, people with diabetes avoid monitoring their blood glucose, and few people monitor their household energy consumption, check their bank balances, keep track of what they are eating and so on. While situational constraints can explain some problems with progress monitoring, we use a self-motives framework to posit that the decision to avoid monitoring often represents the product of an interaction between different motives. For example, the desire to accurately assess progress may conflict with the desire to protect or enhance the self. The present review collates evidence pertaining to the ostrich problem, identifies different motives that underlie the decision to monitor versus not monitor goal progress, illustrates how the ostrich problem might be integrated into models of self-regulation, and provides suggestions for future research. In so doing, the review advances our understanding of the nature and determinants of intentionally deficient monitoring.

[1]  Anne M. Brumbaugh,et al.  The Mediating Role of Feedback Avoidance Behavior in the LMX—Performance Relationship , 2009 .

[2]  George Loewenstein,et al.  The ostrich effect: Selective attention to information , 2009 .

[3]  W. T. Powers Behavior, the control of perception , 1973 .

[4]  Y. Trope,et al.  Reconciling competing motives in self-evaluation: the role of self-control in feedback seeking. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  L. L. Cummings,et al.  FEEDBACK AS AN INDIVIDUAL RESOURCE: PERSONAL STRATEGIES OF CREATING INFORMATION , 1983 .

[6]  Susan J. Ashford,et al.  Reflections on the Looking Glass: A Review of Research on Feedback-Seeking Behavior in Organizations: , 2003 .

[7]  T. Patterson,et al.  Process variables as predictors of risk factor changes in a family health behavior change program. , 1993, Health education research.

[8]  K. Neff,et al.  Self-compassion, Achievement Goals, and Coping with Academic Failure , 2005 .

[9]  Lynn Stockley,et al.  Consumer understanding and use of nutrition labelling: a systematic review , 2005, Public Health Nutrition.

[10]  Frederik Anseel,et al.  How Are We Doing After 30 Years? A Meta-Analytic Review of the Antecedents and Outcomes of Feedback-Seeking Behavior , 2015 .

[11]  M. Ross,et al.  The frequency of temporal-self and social comparisons in people's personal appraisals. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  Enzo Valenzi,et al.  Are You Hiding from Your Boss? The Development of a Taxonomy and Instrument to Assess the Feedback Management Behaviors of Good and Bad Performers , 2003 .

[13]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Determinants of information-seeking behavior , 1979 .

[14]  Frederik Anseel,et al.  A Self-Motives Perspective on Feedback-Seeking Behavior: Linking Organizational Behavior and Social Psychology Research , 2007 .

[15]  R. Prislin,et al.  Motivated Cognitive Processing and Attitude Change , 1998 .

[16]  Gerd Bohner,et al.  Attitudes and attitude change. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  A. Greenwald Self-knowledge and self-deception: Further consideration. , 1997 .

[18]  Erika N. Carlson,et al.  Overcoming the Barriers to Self-Knowledge , 2013, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[19]  C. Carver,et al.  On the Self-Regulation of Behavior , 1998 .

[20]  Aaron C. Kay,et al.  On the perpetuation of ignorance: system dependence, system justification, and the motivated avoidance of sociopolitical information. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  J. Polivy Perception of calories and regulation of intake in restrained and unrestrained subjects , 1976 .

[22]  P. Karoly,et al.  Goal Cognition and Its Clinical Implications , 1995 .

[23]  S. S. van Dam,et al.  Home energy monitors: impact over the medium-term , 2010 .

[24]  Kentaro Fujita,et al.  On Conceptualizing Self-Control as More Than the Effortful Inhibition of Impulses , 2011, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[25]  L. Howie,et al.  Self-Monitoring of Blood Glucose by Adults With Diabetes in the United States Population , 1993, Diabetes Care.

[26]  C. Abraham,et al.  Systematic review of reviews of intervention components associated with increased effectiveness in dietary and physical activity interventions , 2011, BMC public health.

[27]  C. Sedikides,et al.  Quantifying self-motives: functional links between dispositional desires , 2011 .

[28]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Feedback. II. The "credibility gap": delivery of positive and negative and emotional and behavioral feedback in groups. , 1973, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[29]  W A Johnston,et al.  Effect of simulated social feedback on individual tracking performance. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.

[30]  M. Hagger,et al.  Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  Y. Trope,et al.  Resolving Conflicts Among Self-Evaluative Motives: Positive Experiences as a Resource for Overcoming Defensiveness , 1998 .

[32]  Terri L. Griffith,et al.  "Fixed Pie" a la Mode: Information Availability, Information Processing, and the Negotiation of Suboptimal Agreements , 1995 .

[33]  J. Hull A self-awareness model of the causes and effects of alcohol consumption. , 1981, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[34]  M. Strube,et al.  Self-Evaluation: To Thine Own Self Be Good, To Thine Own Self Be Sure, To Thine Own Self Be True, an , 1997 .

[35]  Ying Zhang,et al.  So near and yet so far: the mental representation of goal progress. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Longitudinal improvement of self-regulation through practice: building self-control strength through repeated exercise. , 1999, The Journal of social psychology.

[37]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes , 2006 .

[38]  Margaret Douglas,et al.  Self monitoring of blood glucose in type 2 diabetes: longitudinal qualitative study of patients' perspectives , 2007, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[39]  Wendi A. Miller,et al.  Information Avoidance: Who, What, When, and Why , 2010 .

[40]  Nicolaas P. Pronk,et al.  Self-weighing in weight gain prevention and weight loss trials , 2005, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[41]  C. Vlek,et al.  A review of intervention studies aimed at household energy conservation , 2005 .

[42]  S. Ashford Feedback-Seeking in Individual Adaptation: A Resource Perspective , 1986 .

[43]  Catrine Tudor-Locke,et al.  Health benefits of a pedometer-based physical activity intervention in sedentary workers. , 2004, Preventive medicine.

[44]  C. Carver Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: Placing positive feelings within a general model of affect , 2003, Cognition & emotion.

[45]  N. Liberman,et al.  Normal and pathological consequences of encountering difficulties in monitoring progress toward goals. , 2009 .

[46]  Tom Pyszczynski,et al.  Biased Information Search in the Interpersonal Domain , 1989 .

[47]  Y. Trope Testing self-enhancement and self-assessment theories of achievement motivation: A reply to Sohn's critique , 1986 .

[48]  I. Olkin,et al.  Using pedometers to increase physical activity and improve health: a systematic review. , 2007, JAMA.

[49]  Heidi Grant,et al.  The psychology of goals , 2009 .

[50]  M. Johnston,et al.  Identifying active ingredients in complex behavioural interventions for obese adults with obesity-related co-morbidities or additional risk factors for co-morbidities: a systematic review , 2012 .

[51]  K. Vohs,et al.  Motivation, personal beliefs, and limited resources all contribute to self-control , 2012 .

[52]  Gregory B. Northcraft,et al.  The preservation of self in everyday life: The effects of performance expectations and feedback context on feedback inquiry , 1990 .

[53]  M. Conner,et al.  Can the theory of planned behaviour predict maintenance of a frequently repeated behaviour? , 2007, Psychology, health & medicine.

[54]  Effects of a Self-Monitoring Strategy on Independent Work Behavior of Students with Mild Intellectual Disability. , 2012 .

[55]  Neil Brewer,et al.  The influence of motives and goal orientation on feedback seeking. , 2002 .

[56]  M. Muraven,et al.  Mechanisms of Self-Control Failure: Motivation and Limited Resources , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[57]  P. Sheeran,et al.  Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[58]  L. Candib Why patients who self monitor glucose might be more depressed , 2008, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[59]  S. Karjalainen Consumer preferences for feedback on household electricity consumption , 2011 .

[60]  C. Carver,et al.  Control theory: a useful conceptual framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[61]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation , 1994 .

[62]  Dale H. Schunk,et al.  Effects of Effort Attributional Feedback on Children's Perceived Self-Efficacy and Achievement , 1982 .

[63]  E. Higgins The "self digest": self-knowledge serving self-regulatory functions. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[64]  Ayelet Fishbach,et al.  Tell Me What I Did Wrong: Experts Seek and Respond to Negative Feedback , 2012 .

[65]  K. Vohs,et al.  The Strength Model of Self-Control , 2007, Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine.

[66]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  What Is Ego Depletion? Toward a Mechanistic Revision of the Resource Model of Self-Control , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[67]  G. Miller,et al.  Plans and the structure of behavior , 1960 .

[68]  Rik Pieters,et al.  Dynamics of multiple-goal pursuit. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[69]  J. Polivy,et al.  The effects of self-attention and public attention on eating in restrained and unrestrained subjects. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[70]  Arlen C. Moller,et al.  Choice and Ego-Depletion: The Moderating Role of Autonomy , 2006 .

[71]  C. Carver,et al.  Origins and Functions of Positive and Negative Affect: A Control-Process View. , 1990 .

[72]  Noelle M. Nelson,et al.  Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: a limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[73]  S. Read,et al.  Self-verification processes: How we sustain our self-conceptions , 1981 .

[74]  D. Ruta,et al.  Frequency of blood glucose monitoring in relation to glycaemic control: observational study with diabetes database , 1999, BMJ.

[75]  C. Abraham,et al.  A taxonomy of behavior change techniques used in interventions. , 2008, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[76]  J. Langlois,et al.  Feedback Seeking in Early Adolescence , 2013, The Journal of early adolescence.

[77]  C. Carver,et al.  On the Self-Regulation of Behavior , 1998 .

[78]  Brian Sternthal,et al.  Regulating the Effects of Depletion Through Monitoring , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[79]  Bernhard Schmitz,et al.  Self-monitoring of self-regulation during math homework behaviour using standardized diaries , 2011 .

[80]  Adam R Hafdahl,et al.  Meta-analysis of patient education interventions to increase physical activity among chronically ill adults. , 2008, Patient education and counseling.

[81]  R V Hall,et al.  The effect of self-recording on the classroom behavior of two eighth-grade students. , 1971, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[82]  A. Kluger,et al.  The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. , 1996 .

[83]  P. Sheeran Intention—Behavior Relations: A Conceptual and Empirical Review , 2002 .

[84]  D. Schallert,et al.  Self-kindness when facing stress: The role of self-compassion, goal regulation, and support in college students’ well-being , 2009 .

[85]  Charles R. Berger,et al.  Strategic and Nonstrategic Information Acquisition , 2002 .

[86]  Frederik Anseel,et al.  Understanding and encouraging feedback‐seeking behaviour: a literature review , 2013, Medical education.

[87]  W. T. Powers,et al.  A General Feedback Theory of Human Behavior: Part I , 1960 .

[88]  Brian P. Meier,et al.  Motivational Versus Metabolic Effects of Carbohydrates on Self-Control , 2012, Psychological science.

[89]  C. Watts,et al.  The IMAGE Study Group , 2005 .

[90]  M. Strube,et al.  Self-evaluation of abilities: accurate self-assessment versus biased self-enhancement. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[91]  Ayelet Fishbach,et al.  CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Self-Control A Function of Knowing When and How to Exercise Restraint , 2022 .

[92]  C. Abraham,et al.  Effective techniques in healthy eating and physical activity interventions: a meta-regression. , 2009, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[93]  M. Conner,et al.  Predicting health behaviour : research and practice with social cognition models , 2005 .