Perceived Structure of Triads

This study explored the perception of major, minor, diminished, and augmented triads in two experiments. In the first, subjects judged whether pairs of these triads were of the same or different chord type. In the second, subject rated the similarity of these pairs of triads. Subjects were grouped by degree of musical training. We looked for effects of several variables on the subjects9 judgments: the "musical key" suggested by major and minor triads, whether a triad was presented in root position or in first inversion, and whether the notes of the triad were presented successively (i. e., arpeggiated) or simultaneously. Among the three groups of subjects, only the judgments of those who were highly trained appeared to be influenced by the above factors. For these subjects, judgments were influenced by relative position in the circle of fifths. In addition, judgments appeared to incorporate the perceived degree of consonance or dissonance, rather than relative interval sizes.

[1]  D J Mewhort,et al.  Perception of structure in short melodic sequences. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  S. C. Johnson Hierarchical clustering schemes , 1967, Psychometrika.

[3]  Annabel J. Cohen,et al.  Recognition of Transposed Melodic Sequences , 1976 .

[4]  C. Krumhansl The psychological representation of musical pitch in a tonal context , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Consonance and dissonance judgments of musical chords , 1983 .

[6]  E. Guthrie,et al.  The Fusion of Non-Musical Intervals , 1928 .

[7]  D. Mewhort,et al.  Recognition memory for single tones with and without context. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[8]  R. Shepard 11 – Structural Representations of Musical Pitch , 1982 .

[9]  R. Plomp,et al.  Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  W J Levelt,et al.  Triadic comparisons of musical intervals. , 1966, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[11]  E. Terhardt Psychoacoustic evaluation of musical sounds , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  R. Plomp,et al.  The connotation of musical consonance , 1962 .

[13]  H. Schlosberg,et al.  Octave generalization, pitch discrimination, and loudness thresholds in the white rat. , 1943 .

[14]  C. Krumhansl,et al.  Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. , 1982 .

[15]  L. Cuddy,et al.  Melody recognition: the experimental application of musical rules. , 1979, Canadian journal of psychology.

[16]  Juan G. Roederer,et al.  Introduction to the physics and psychophysics of music , 1973 .

[17]  W. Dowling Scale and contour: Two components of a theory of memory for melodies. , 1978 .

[18]  W. Jay Dowling,et al.  Recognition of transposed melodies: a key-distance effect in developmental perspective. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  R. Shepard The analysis of proximities: Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function. II , 1962 .

[20]  Thomas B. Jeffries Relationship of interval frequency count to ratings of melodic intervals. , 1974 .

[21]  P. E. Stopp Frequency analysis and periodicity detection in hearing 1971, Plomp and Smoorenburg (Editors). Leiden, Netherlands: Sijthoff Leiden. Cloth, Fl. 60 , 1971 .

[22]  C. Krumhansl,et al.  The representation of harmonic structure in music: Hierarchies of stability as a function of context , 1983, Cognition.

[23]  David Allen,et al.  Octave discriminability of musical and non-musical subjects , 1967 .

[24]  F. Attneave,et al.  Pitch as a medium: a new approach to psychophysical scaling. , 1971, The American journal of psychology.

[25]  L. Humphreys Generalization as a function of method of reinforcement. , 1939 .

[26]  A. Kameoka,et al.  Consonance theory part II: consonance of complex tones and its calculation method. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  柴田 南雄,et al.  ニューグローヴ世界音楽大事典 = The new Grove dictionary of music and musicians , 1879 .

[28]  E. Terhardt Pitch, consonance, and harmony. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  Paul Lorton,et al.  Interval Recognition: Identification of Harmonic and Melodic Intervals , 1975 .

[30]  E. B. Silverman,et al.  A multidimensional scaling analysis of 16 complex sounds , 1976 .

[31]  L. Stein Structure and Style , 1963 .

[32]  A. Kameoka,et al.  Consonance theory part I: consonance of dyads. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  R. Shepard,et al.  Quantification of the Hierarchy of Tonal Functions Within a Diatonic Context , 1979 .

[34]  Gerald J. Balzano On the bases of similarity of musical intervals: a chronometric analysis , 1977 .

[35]  Ernst Terhardt,et al.  Calculating virtual pitch , 1979, Hearing Research.

[36]  A. Stewart,et al.  The time course of lateral asymmetries in visual perception of letters. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[37]  E. Terhardt,et al.  Pitch of complex signals according to virtual‐pitch theory: Tests, examples, and predictions , 1982 .

[38]  E. F. Evans,et al.  Psychophysics and Physiology of Hearing , 1979 .

[39]  R. Shepard Circularity in Judgments of Relative Pitch , 1964 .