AAC menu interface: Effectiveness of active versus passive learning to master abbreviation-expansion codes

This study investigated the accuracy with which 30 young adults without disabilities learned abbreviation expansion codes associated with specific vocabulary items that were stored in an AAC device with two accessing methods: mouse access and keyboard access. Both accessing methods utilized a specialized computer application, called AAC Menu, which allowed for errorless practice. Mouse access prompted passive learning, whereas keyboard access prompted active learning. Results revealed that participants who accessed words via a keyboard demonstrated significantly higher mastery of abbreviation-expansion codes than those who accessed words via a computer mouse.

[1]  Charles G. Gross,et al.  CAUDATE, CORTICAL, HIPPOCAMPAL AND DORSAL THALAMIC LESIONS IN RATS: ALTERNATION AND HEBB-WILLIAMS MAZE PERFORMANCE , 1965 .

[2]  Jonathan J. Evans,et al.  A Comparison of ''Errorless'' and ''Trial-and-error'' Learning Methods for Teaching Individuals with Acquired Memory Deficits , 2000 .

[3]  Janice Light,et al.  The effects of message encoding techniques on recall by literate adults using AAC systems , 1990 .

[4]  Ann R. Beck,et al.  Can People Who Have Aphasia Learn Iconic Codes , 1998 .

[5]  David R. Beukelman,et al.  Communication augmentation : a casebook of clinical management , 1985 .

[6]  Kathryn D. R. Drager,et al.  Performance of Typically Developing Four- and Five-Year-Old Children with AAC Systems using Different Language Organization Techniques , 2004 .

[7]  K. Hux,et al.  Augmentative and Alternative Communication use and acceptance by adults with Traumatic Brain Injury , 2006, Augmentative and alternative communication.

[8]  J B Angelo A comparison of three coding methods for abbreviation expansion in acceleration vocabularies , 1990, International journal of rehabilitation research. Internationale Zeitschrift fur Rehabilitationsforschung. Revue internationale de recherches de readaptation.

[9]  B R Baker,et al.  Using images to generate speech , 1986 .

[10]  D. Beukelman,et al.  Augmentative & Alternative Communication: Supporting Children & Adults With Complex Communication Needs , 2006 .

[11]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .

[12]  Sarah W. Blackstone,et al.  Augmentative communication : an introduction , 1986 .

[13]  John Todman,et al.  Availability and Accessibility in Whole Utterance-Based Devices , 2001 .

[14]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impaired people , 1994 .

[15]  Chih-Kang Yang Development of an augmentative and alternative communication interface for persons with memory and new learning limitations , 2000 .

[16]  M. Mimura,et al.  Errorless and Effortful Processes Involved in the Learning of Face-name Associations by Patients with Alcoholic Korsakoff's Syndrome , 2000 .

[17]  J. Elliot,et al.  Communication interaction differences communication between natural speakers and AAC users with traumatic brain injury , 2001 .

[18]  N. Hunkin,et al.  Are the benefits of errorless learning dependent on implicit memory? , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  Barbara A. Wilson,et al.  When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination , 1994, Neuropsychologia.