Vocal interactions at the dawn of communication: The emergence of mutuality and complementarity in mother-infant interaction

Continuous interaction of mother and infant in the first weeks and months of an infant's life entrains the infant on many crucial aspects of how to do things together. Contingencies of gaze, vocalizations, and other movements are slowly routinized; this scaffolds directing of attention to each other and the world and gives to such multimodal interactions meaning. It is within these continuous interactions with caregivers that language emerges, starting from the first non-reflexive vocalizations that infants produce. The response that caregivers promptly give to these vocalizations informs infants of their relevance and helps shape them. We explored this systematicity by observing the coupling of infants' and mothers' vocalizations in unconstrained interactions longitudinally. While at three months, mothers seem to answer consistently to any speech related vocalization within the first two seconds, this pattern fades away at six and eight months. What remains stable across age is a structure in which overlapping vocalizations are rare and give way to a sequential pattern of vocal reciprocity — an embryonic turn-taking behavior. Discussion relates this finding to early coordination in other modalities in an attempt to sketch a more holistic account of emerging co-action.

[1]  Riccardo Fusaroli,et al.  Dialog as interpersonal synergy , 2014, New Ideas in Psychology.

[2]  Jill Gilkerson,et al.  A Social Feedback Loop for Speech Development and Its Reduction in Autism , 2014, Psychological science.

[3]  M. Goldstein,et al.  Social Feedback to Infants' Babbling Facilitates Rapid Phonological Learning , 2008, Psychological science.

[4]  Andrew P. King,et al.  Mothers provide differential feedback to infants' prelinguistic sounds , 2006 .

[5]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat: doing phonetics by computer , 2003 .

[6]  A. Fogel Developing through relationships , 1993 .

[7]  Katharina J. Rohlfing,et al.  Language Does Something: Body Action and Language in Maternal Input to Three-Month-Olds , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[8]  Heidi Keller,et al.  Temporal Contingency as an Independent Component of Parenting Behavior , 1999 .

[9]  Katharina J. Rohlfing,et al.  How can multimodal cues from child-directed interaction reduce learning complexity in robots? , 2006, Adv. Robotics.

[10]  Clément Moulin-Frier,et al.  Autonomous development of turn-taking behaviors in agent populations: A computational study , 2015, 2015 Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-EpiRob).

[11]  E. D. Paolo,et al.  An enactive approach to social cognition , 2007 .

[12]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[13]  M. Goldstein,et al.  Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  The emergence of language. , 1999 .

[15]  Darshana Narayanan,et al.  A computational model for vocal exchange dynamics and their development in marmoset monkeys , 2012, 2012 IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL).

[16]  Minoru Asada,et al.  Modeling Early Vocal Development Through Infant–Caregiver Interaction: A Review , 2016, IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems.

[17]  Jenny R. Saffran,et al.  Statistical learning mechanisms in infancy , 2020, Neural Circuit and Cognitive Development.

[18]  Elma E. Hilbrink,et al.  Early developmental changes in the timing of turn-taking: a longitudinal study of mother–infant interaction , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[19]  Richard Moore Learning to do things with words , 2009 .

[20]  J. Bruner Child's Talk: Learning to Use Language , 1985 .

[21]  E. D. Paolo,et al.  Participatory sense-making , 2007 .

[22]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Nominal Cross Recurrence as a Generalized Lag Sequential Analysis for Behavioral Streams , 2011, Int. J. Bifurc. Chaos.

[23]  A. Fogel,et al.  Stability and transitions in mother-infant face-to-face communication during the first 6 months: a microhistorical approach. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[24]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  Six views of embodied cognition , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  Iris Nomikou,et al.  Constructing Interaction: The Development of Gaze Dynamics , 2016 .

[26]  Christine M. Johnson,et al.  Sensorimotor Decoupling Contributes to Triadic Attention: A Longitudinal Investigation of Mother-Infant-Object Interactions. , 2016, Child development.

[27]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  Giulio Sandini,et al.  In Press, Ieee Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development , 2010 .

[29]  A. Lock Action, gesture, and symbol : the emergence of language , 1978 .

[30]  Mark Steedman The Emergence of Language , 2017 .

[31]  Katharina J. Rohlfing,et al.  Young Children’s Dialogical Actions: The Beginnings of Purposeful Intersubjectivity , 2013, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[32]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[33]  D. Lewkowicz Early experience and multisensory perceptual narrowing. , 2014, Developmental psychobiology.

[34]  Jürgen Kurths,et al.  Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems , 2009 .

[35]  S. Feldstein,et al.  Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: coordinated timing in development. , 2001, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[36]  Marc H Bornstein,et al.  The value of vocalizing: five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. , 2009, Child development.

[37]  H. D. Jaegher,et al.  Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation , 2009 .

[38]  Pierre-Yves Oudeyer,et al.  An Alternative to Mapping a Word onto a Concept in Language Acquisition: Pragmatic Frames , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[39]  Katharina Rohlfing,et al.  Intermodal synchrony as a form of maternal responsiveness: association with language development , 2014 .

[40]  M. Gratier,et al.  Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[41]  Malte Schilling,et al.  Language-at all times: Action and interaction as contexts for enriching representations , 2016 .

[42]  Moreno I. Coco,et al.  Cross-recurrence quantification analysis of categorical and continuous time series: an R package , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[43]  C. Trevarthen Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity , 1979 .