Eye position specificity of saccadic adaptation.

PURPOSE The accuracy of saccadic eye movements is maintained throughout life by adaptive mechanisms. With the double-step target paradigm, eight human subjects were investigated to determine whether saccadic adaptation depends only on the eye-displacement vector, or also on eye position as a context cue when two saccades of identical vector are adapted simultaneously. METHODS First, bidirectional adaptations (BDAs) of horizontal saccades of the same vector were induced in a single training phase. Each direction of adaptation in BDAs (backward and forward) was linked to one vertical eye position (e.g., forward adaptation performed with the eyes directed 12.5 degrees upward and backward adaptation with the eyes 25 degrees downward) and alternated from trial to trial. Second, unidirectional adaptations (UDAs) were tested in two control conditions in which training trials of a single direction (backward or forward) were presented at both 12.5 degrees and -25 degrees eye elevations. RESULTS Opposite changes in saccade amplitude could develop simultaneously in BDA, indicating that saccadic adaptation depends on orbital eye position. Comparing these data with the control conditions further indicated that eye position specificity was complete for backward, but not for forward, adaptation. CONCLUSIONS The results indicate that saccadic adaptation mechanisms use vectorial eye displacement signals, but can also take eye position signals into account as a contextual cue when the training involves conflicting saccade amplitude changes.

[1]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Control of saccadic eye movements and combined eye/head gaze shifts by the medio-posterior cerebellum. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[2]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Adaptation of Saccadic Eye Movements: Transfer and Specificity , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[3]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Characteristics of saccadic gain adaptation in rhesus macaques. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  P. Thier,et al.  Saccadic Dysmetria and Adaptation after Lesions of the Cerebellar Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Sensory, motor, and combined contexts for context-specific adaptation of saccade gain in humans , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[6]  L. Optican,et al.  Adaptive response to ocular muscle weakness in human pursuit and saccadic eye movements. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  T Anstis,et al.  Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  C. J. Erkelens,et al.  Selective adaptation of internally triggered saccades made to visual targets , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  S. C. Mclaughlin Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements , 1967 .

[10]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional adaptation of reactive saccades in humans: a PET study , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  H. Deubel Is saccadic adaptation context-specific? , 1995 .

[12]  L. Ritchie Effects of cerebellar lesions on saccadic eye movements. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Context-specific adaptation of saccade gain , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  D. Zee,et al.  Context-specific adaptation of pursuit initiation in humans. , 2000, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[15]  A. Opstal,et al.  Transfer of short-term adaptation in human saccadic eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  J. A. Gisbergen,et al.  Specificity of saccadic adaptation in three-dimensional space , 1997, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Vercher,et al.  Mechanisms of short-term saccadic adaptation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  D. Robinson,et al.  Context-specific adaptation of the gain of the vestibulo-ocular reflex in humans. , 1992, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[19]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Eye movement research : mechanisms, processes and applications , 1995 .

[20]  Christopher T. Noto,et al.  Characteristics of simian adaptation fields produced by behavioral changes in saccade size and direction. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  H. Deubel ADAPTIVITY OF GAIN AND DIRECTION IN OBLIQUE SACCADES1 , 1987 .

[22]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Role of the caudal fastigial nucleus in saccade generation. II. Effects of muscimol inactivation. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Masahiko Fujita,et al.  Selective and delay adaptation of human saccades. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[24]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Flexibility of saccade adaptation in the monkey: different gain states for saccades in the same direction , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  L. Optican,et al.  Cerebellar-dependent adaptive control of primate saccadic system. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  J. E. Albano AdaptiveChangesin Saccade Oculocentricor Orbitocentric Amplitude: Mapping? , 1997 .

[27]  H. Deubel Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements , 1995, Vision Research.

[29]  H Deubel,et al.  Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. , 1986, Human neurobiology.

[30]  Clifton Schor,et al.  Spatial aspects of vertical phoria adaptation , 1993, Vision Research.

[31]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Genetic dissection of Alzheimer's disease and related dementias: amyloid and its relationship to tau , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  D. Zee,et al.  Effects of lesions of the oculomotor vermis on eye movements in primate: saccades. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.