How the brain resolves high conflict situations: Double conflict involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

Executive control is a human ability that allows to overcome automatic stimulus-response mappings and to act appropriate in the context of a task where the selection of relevant stimuli and the suppression of interfering information are crucial. In order to address the question which brain areas are involved in the detection and processing of two simultaneously operating sources of interference derived from a spatial incompatibility task, we used functional MRI to contrast neural activity related to a double conflict situation to single incompatibility conditions. Results show signal increase of left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex when monitoring simultaneously presented conflict. There was no additional activity in the medial prefrontal cortex or anterior cingulate cortex although these regions are expected to play an important role in all types of conflict monitoring. Further analyses of conflict resolution and post-error adaptation pointed to different underlying functional mechanisms. While the resolution of high conflict was associated with rostral ACC activation, the post-error adaptation reflecting activity during post-error trials suggests a specific medial and lateral prefrontal network which was functionally distinct from conflict-related activity. Our results also suggest a major role for the basal ganglia during error detection and resolution.

[1]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Individual differences in error processing: a review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI studies using the GO/NOGO task. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  J R Simon,et al.  Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the 'Stroop effect' vs. the 'Simon effect'. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[5]  E. Stoffels On stage robustness and response selection routes: Further evidence , 1996 .

[6]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  Manfred Herrmann,et al.  Comparison of two Simon tasks: Neuronal correlates of conflict resolution based on coherent motion perception , 2006, NeuroImage.

[8]  Xun Liu,et al.  Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[9]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[10]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[11]  Tor D Wager,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: a meta-analysis , 2004, NeuroImage.

[12]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[13]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[14]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  Gernot D. Kleiter,et al.  Beyond the answer: Post-error processes , 1989, Cognition.

[16]  Peter Wühr,et al.  Evidence for gating of direct response activation in the Simon task. , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  E. Kandel,et al.  Resolving Emotional Conflict: A Role for the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Modulating Activity in the Amygdala , 2006, Neuron.

[18]  Elisa Ciaramelli,et al.  The Regulation of Cognitive Control following Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex Lesion in Humans , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  John C Gore,et al.  An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[20]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[21]  E. Kandel,et al.  Resolving Emotional Conflict: A Role for the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Modulating Activit , 2006 .

[22]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[23]  Lauren M. Bylsma,et al.  The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[24]  S Kornblum,et al.  The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli , 1994, Psychological research.

[25]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[27]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[28]  Paul J. Laurienti,et al.  An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets , 2003, NeuroImage.

[29]  T. Egner Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[31]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[32]  Tobias Egner,et al.  The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task , 2005, NeuroImage.

[33]  Thomas E. Hazy,et al.  Towards an executive without a homunculus: computational models of the prefrontal cortex/basal ganglia system , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[35]  A. Schnitzler,et al.  Magnetic stimulation of the dorsal premotor cortex modulates the Simon effect. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[36]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Interactions between stimulus-stimulus congruence and stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .

[37]  Carol L. Colby,et al.  Stimulus–Response Incompatibility Activates Cortex Proximate to Three Eye Fields , 2001, NeuroImage.

[38]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  Control over response priming in visuomotor processing: a lateralized event-related potential study , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain , 2007, NeuroImage.

[41]  U. Ansorge,et al.  Exploring trial-by-trial modulations of the Simon effect , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[42]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  E. Soetens,et al.  Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2002, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[45]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  The Role of Intact Frontostriatal Circuits in Error Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.

[47]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[48]  C. Liston,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[49]  R. Veit,et al.  Regulation of emotional responses elicited by threat‐related stimuli , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[50]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[51]  J. Ashe,et al.  The Effect of Stimulus–Response Compatibility on Cortical Motor Activation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[52]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Brain-behavior relationships: evidence from practice effects in spatial stimulus-response compatibility. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  D. Bradley,et al.  Structure and function of visual area MT. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[55]  Jin Fan,et al.  Cognitive and Brain Consequences of Conflict , 2003, NeuroImage.

[56]  S. Nieuwenhuis,et al.  Neural mechanisms of attention and control: losing our inhibitions? , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[58]  H Garavan,et al.  A midline dissociation between error-processing and response-conflict monitoring , 2003, NeuroImage.

[60]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[61]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Neural implementation of response selection in humans as revealed by localized effects of stimulus–response compatibility on brain activation , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[62]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Sequential modulations of interference evoked by processing task-irrelevant stimulus features. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[63]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[64]  G. Glover,et al.  Error‐related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[65]  M. Brass,et al.  Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: Meta‐analyses of switching and Stroop studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.