Observational practice of relative but not absolute motion features in a single-limb multi-joint coordination task

The learning of relative and absolute motion features as a function of physical (actor group) and observational (observer group) practice was examined in a rhythmic single limb multi-joint coordination task. The task required the participants to learn a 90° relative phase pattern between the elbow and wrist in combination with an absolute elbow joint angle of 80° and a wrist joint angle of 48°. Each actor practiced the required relative and absolute motion features for 2 days while being watched by an observer. Overall, the actor group was characterized by an improvement in performance on the relative phase component and showed a clear differentiation in joint amplitudes. In a 24-h retention test, the observer group more closely matched the performance of the actors on the relative phase component in comparison to a control group that was not exposed to physical or observational practice. However, the observer and control groups did not demonstrate a clear differentiation in required joint amplitudes. In agreement with Scully and Newell (1985), we conclude that relative phase may be classified as a relative motion feature that may be picked through observation and benefit initial physical performance, whereas the joint amplitudes may be classified as absolute motion features that require physical practice to achieve the appropriate scaling.

[1]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[2]  Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer across different effector systems. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  S. Bennett,et al.  Observational Modeling Effects for Movement Dynamics and Movement Outcome Measures Across Differing Task Constraints: A Meta-Analysis , 2006, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  T D Lee,et al.  Learning a new bimanual coordination pattern: reciprocal influences of intrinsic and to-be-learned patterns. , 1997, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[5]  D L Wright,et al.  Can Observational Practice Facilitate Error Recognition and Movement Production? , 2000, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[6]  J. Kelso,et al.  Learning as change of coordination dynamics: theory and experiment. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  Order parameters for the neural organization of single, multijoint limb movement patterns , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990 .

[9]  J A Kelso,et al.  Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. , 1988, Science.

[10]  A. Williams,et al.  Visual perception and gaze control in judging versus producing phase relations. , 2005, Human movement science.

[11]  D L Wright,et al.  Learning and Transfer of a Relative Phase Pattern and a Joint Amplitude Ratio in a Rhythmic Multijoint Arm Movement , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  H. Haken,et al.  A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[14]  Ian M. Franks,et al.  Attention focusing instructions and coordination bias : implications for learning a novel bimanual task , 2000 .

[15]  J. A. S. Kelso,et al.  Relative timing in brain and behavior: Some observations about the generalized motor program and self-organized coordination dynamics , 1997 .

[16]  Romeo Chua,et al.  The Role of Video in Facilitating Perception and Action of a Novel Coordination Movement , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[17]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Self-organization of trajectory formation , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[18]  R. H. Wimmers,et al.  Phase transitions in rhythmic tracking movements: A case of unilateral coupling , 1992 .

[19]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Effects of an Auditory Model on the Learning of Relative and Absolute Timing , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[20]  J. J. Buchanan,et al.  Self-organization of trajectory formation , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[21]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  A Perceptually Driven Dynamical Model of Bimanual Rhythmic Movement (and Phase Perception) , 2004 .

[22]  B Abernethy,et al.  Note: A statistical problem in testing invariance of movement using the phase plane model. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative Phase Alterations During Bimanual Skill Acquisition. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  David R. Collins,et al.  Human movement coordination implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase , 2010 .

[25]  S. Vogt,et al.  On relations between perceiving, imagining and performing in the learning of cyclical movement sequences. , 1995, British journal of psychology.

[26]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations , 1986 .

[27]  G. Churchill Fundamentals of experimental design for cDNA microarrays , 2002, Nature Genetics.

[28]  J. Summers,et al.  The Dynamics of Bimanual Circle Drawing , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[29]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: collective and component levels. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  David L Wright,et al.  Learning to Detect Error in Movement Timing Using Physical and Observational Practice , 2005, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[31]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Hierarchical control of different elbow-wrist coordination patterns , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  A. Williams,et al.  What is modelled during observational learning? , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[33]  J. Kelso,et al.  A quantitative approach to understanding the formation and change of coordinated movement patterns. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  P G Zanone,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  A Bandura,et al.  Translating cognition into action: the role of visual guidance in observational learning. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[36]  J. Kelso,et al.  The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics , 1994 .

[37]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  A Bandura,et al.  Role of timing of visual monitoring and motor rehearsal in observational learning of action patterns. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.

[39]  J. A. S. Kelso,et al.  Dynamic pattern generation and recognition , 1991 .

[40]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: Critical slowing down and switching time , 1987 .

[41]  J. Kelso,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .

[42]  S. Coren The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: Norms for young adults , 1993 .

[43]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Learning as a function of coordination bias: building upon pre-practice behaviours. , 2002, Human movement science.

[44]  J. Kelso,et al.  Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  I M Franks,et al.  Learning a Coordination Skill: Interactive Effects of Instruction and Feedback , 2001, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[46]  Arnaud Badets,et al.  Intention in motor learning through observation , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[47]  Polemnia G. Amazeen,et al.  Is Dynamics the Content of a Generalized Motor Program for Rhythmic Interlimb Coordination? , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[48]  J. Kelso,et al.  A synergetic theory of environmentally-specified and learned patterns of movement coordination , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[49]  J. Cutting,et al.  The minimum principle and the perception of absolute, common, and relative motions , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[50]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  S. Vogt,et al.  From visuo-motor interactions to imitation learning: Behavioural and brain imaging studies , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[52]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[53]  Andrew A G Mattar,et al.  Motor Learning by Observing , 2005, Neuron.

[54]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer across different effector systems. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  J. Kelso,et al.  A synergetic theory of environmentally-specified and learned patterns of movement coordination , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[56]  J. L. Myers Fundamentals of Experimental Design , 1972 .

[57]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[58]  A Bandura,et al.  The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: making the unobservable observable. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[59]  Simon J. Bennett,et al.  Effects of manipulating relative and absolute motion information during observational learning of an aiming task , 2001, Journal of sports sciences.

[60]  John J. Buchanan,et al.  Learning a single limb multijoint coordination pattern: the impact of a mechanical constraint on the coordination dynamics of learning and transfer , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.