No pain, no gain: the affective valence of congruency conditions changes following a successful response
暂无分享,去创建一个
W. Notebaert | Massimo Silvetti | T. Verguts | S. Braem | Nathalie Schouppe | J. Houwer | K. Ridderinkhof
[1] I. Koch,et al. Mood states influence cognitive control: the case of conflict adaptation , 2014, Psychological Research.
[2] Kae Nakamura,et al. Predictive Reward Signal of Dopamine Neurons , 2015 .
[3] G. Dreisbach,et al. The time course of the aversive conflict signal. , 2015, Experimental psychology.
[4] Elger L. Abrahamse,et al. The Congruency Sequence Effect 3.0: A Critical Test of Conflict Adaptation , 2014, PloS one.
[5] W. Notebaert,et al. The heterogeneous world of congruency sequence effects: an update , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[6] James R. Schmidt,et al. Congruency Sequence Effects without Feature Integration or Contingency Learning Confounds , 2014, PloS one.
[7] Yang Seok Cho,et al. Congruency sequence effect without feature integration and contingency learning. , 2014, Acta psychologica.
[8] W. Notebaert,et al. Disentangling posterror and postconflict reduction of interference , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[9] Wim Notebaert,et al. Context-specific control and context selection in conflict tasks. , 2014, Acta psychologica.
[10] Franziska M. Korb,et al. Affective Modulation of Cognitive Control is Determined by Performance-Contingency and Mediated by Ventromedial Prefrontal and Cingulate Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[11] K. Berridge,et al. Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[12] James R. Schmidt. Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[13] G. Dreisbach,et al. Conflicts as aversive signals: Conflict priming increases negative judgments for neutral stimuli , 2013, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[14] Trevor Coward,et al. Nova Science Publishers , 2013 .
[15] W. Notebaert,et al. Reward modulates adaptations to conflict , 2012, Cognition.
[16] Punit Shah. Toward a Neurobiology of Unrealistic Optimism , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[17] G. Pourtois,et al. Evidence for the automatic evaluation of self-generated actions , 2012, Cognition.
[18] Mark A. Elliott,et al. Being right is its own reward: Load and performance related ventral striatum activation to correct responses during a working memory task in youth , 2012, NeuroImage.
[19] J. T. Mordkoff. Observation: Three reasons to avoid having half of the trials be congruent in a four-alternative forced-choice experiment on sequential modulation , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[20] Michael X. Cohen,et al. Theta Dynamics Reveal Domain-specific Control over Stimulus and Response Conflict , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] W. Notebaert,et al. Conflict: Run! Reduced Stroop interference with avoidance responses , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[22] G. Dreisbach,et al. Conflicts as aversive signals , 2012, Brain and Cognition.
[23] Margaret T. Lynn,et al. The Phenomenology of Quitting , 2012 .
[24] Michael Zehetleitner,et al. Dissociable Effects of Valence and Arousal in Adaptive Executive Control , 2011, PloS one.
[25] W. Sommer,et al. Reward and Punishment Effects on Error Processing and Conflict Control , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[26] Laurent P. Ferrier,et al. Action compatibility effects are hedonically marked and have incidental consequences on affective judgment. , 2011, Emotion.
[27] James R. Schmidt,et al. Now you see it, now you don't: controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions eliminates the Gratton effect. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[28] Joshua W. Brown,et al. Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[29] Massimo Silvetti,et al. Value and Prediction Error in Medial Frontal Cortex: Integrating the Single-Unit and Systems Levels of Analysis , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[30] R. Davidson,et al. The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[31] W. Notebaert,et al. Conflict and error adaptation in the Simon task. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[32] T. Zentall. Justification of Effort by Humans and Pigeons , 2010 .
[33] S. Tipper,et al. Sensorimotor fluency influences affect: Evidence from electromyography , 2010 .
[34] G. Band,et al. Reward Counteracts Conflict Adaptation , 2009, Psychological science.
[35] Wim Notebaert,et al. Adaptation by binding: a learning account of cognitive control , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[36] Jean-Claude Darcheville,et al. Preference for rewards that follow greater effort and greater delay , 2008, Learning & behavior.
[37] M. Botvinick. Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[38] Mike Wendt,et al. Disentangling Sequential Effects of Stimulus- and Response-related Conflict and Stimulus-Response Repetition using Brain Potentials , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[39] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[40] Wim Notebaert,et al. Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[41] G. Dreisbach. How positive affect modulates cognitive control: The costs and benefits of reduced maintenance capability , 2006, Brain and Cognition.
[42] Cameron S. Carter,et al. Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: A functional MRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.
[43] Thomas R Zentall,et al. Contrast and the justification of effort , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[44] G. Dreisbach,et al. How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[45] E. Awh,et al. Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[46] Jan De Houwer,et al. On the role of stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect , 2003 .
[47] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[48] R. Fazio. On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview , 2001 .
[49] B. Hommel. Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .
[50] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[51] D. Hermans,et al. Affective and subjective familiarity ratings of 740 Dutch words , 1994 .
[52] A. Henik,et al. Controlling Stroop effects by manipulating expectations for color words , 1992, Memory & cognition.
[53] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[54] R. Eisenberger. Learned industriousness. , 1992, Psychological review.
[55] D. Norman,et al. Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .
[56] Willis F. Overton,et al. Advances in research and theory , 1977 .
[57] G. Schwartz,et al. Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .
[58] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[59] Richard C. Atkinson,et al. Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes , 1968, Psychology of Learning and Motivation.