No pain, no gain: the affective valence of congruency conditions changes following a successful response

[1]  I. Koch,et al.  Mood states influence cognitive control: the case of conflict adaptation , 2014, Psychological Research.

[2]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Predictive Reward Signal of Dopamine Neurons , 2015 .

[3]  G. Dreisbach,et al.  The time course of the aversive conflict signal. , 2015, Experimental psychology.

[4]  Elger L. Abrahamse,et al.  The Congruency Sequence Effect 3.0: A Critical Test of Conflict Adaptation , 2014, PloS one.

[5]  W. Notebaert,et al.  The heterogeneous world of congruency sequence effects: an update , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[6]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  Congruency Sequence Effects without Feature Integration or Contingency Learning Confounds , 2014, PloS one.

[7]  Yang Seok Cho,et al.  Congruency sequence effect without feature integration and contingency learning. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[8]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Disentangling posterror and postconflict reduction of interference , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  Context-specific control and context selection in conflict tasks. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[10]  Franziska M. Korb,et al.  Affective Modulation of Cognitive Control is Determined by Performance-Contingency and Mediated by Ventromedial Prefrontal and Cingulate Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  K. Berridge,et al.  Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  James R. Schmidt Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[13]  G. Dreisbach,et al.  Conflicts as aversive signals: Conflict priming increases negative judgments for neutral stimuli , 2013, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[14]  Trevor Coward,et al.  Nova Science Publishers , 2013 .

[15]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Reward modulates adaptations to conflict , 2012, Cognition.

[16]  Punit Shah Toward a Neurobiology of Unrealistic Optimism , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[17]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Evidence for the automatic evaluation of self-generated actions , 2012, Cognition.

[18]  Mark A. Elliott,et al.  Being right is its own reward: Load and performance related ventral striatum activation to correct responses during a working memory task in youth , 2012, NeuroImage.

[19]  J. T. Mordkoff Observation: Three reasons to avoid having half of the trials be congruent in a four-alternative forced-choice experiment on sequential modulation , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[20]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Theta Dynamics Reveal Domain-specific Control over Stimulus and Response Conflict , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Conflict: Run! Reduced Stroop interference with avoidance responses , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  G. Dreisbach,et al.  Conflicts as aversive signals , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[23]  Margaret T. Lynn,et al.  The Phenomenology of Quitting , 2012 .

[24]  Michael Zehetleitner,et al.  Dissociable Effects of Valence and Arousal in Adaptive Executive Control , 2011, PloS one.

[25]  W. Sommer,et al.  Reward and Punishment Effects on Error Processing and Conflict Control , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[26]  Laurent P. Ferrier,et al.  Action compatibility effects are hedonically marked and have incidental consequences on affective judgment. , 2011, Emotion.

[27]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  Now you see it, now you don't: controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions eliminates the Gratton effect. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[28]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  Massimo Silvetti,et al.  Value and Prediction Error in Medial Frontal Cortex: Integrating the Single-Unit and Systems Levels of Analysis , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[30]  R. Davidson,et al.  The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Conflict and error adaptation in the Simon task. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[32]  T. Zentall Justification of Effort by Humans and Pigeons , 2010 .

[33]  S. Tipper,et al.  Sensorimotor fluency influences affect: Evidence from electromyography , 2010 .

[34]  G. Band,et al.  Reward Counteracts Conflict Adaptation , 2009, Psychological science.

[35]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  Adaptation by binding: a learning account of cognitive control , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Jean-Claude Darcheville,et al.  Preference for rewards that follow greater effort and greater delay , 2008, Learning & behavior.

[37]  M. Botvinick Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[38]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Disentangling Sequential Effects of Stimulus- and Response-related Conflict and Stimulus-Response Repetition using Brain Potentials , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[40]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  G. Dreisbach How positive affect modulates cognitive control: The costs and benefits of reduced maintenance capability , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[42]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: A functional MRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[43]  Thomas R Zentall,et al.  Contrast and the justification of effort , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  G. Dreisbach,et al.  How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  On the role of stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect , 2003 .

[47]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[48]  R. Fazio On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview , 2001 .

[49]  B. Hommel Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .

[50]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[51]  D. Hermans,et al.  Affective and subjective familiarity ratings of 740 Dutch words , 1994 .

[52]  A. Henik,et al.  Controlling Stroop effects by manipulating expectations for color words , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[53]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[54]  R. Eisenberger Learned industriousness. , 1992, Psychological review.

[55]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[56]  Willis F. Overton,et al.  Advances in research and theory , 1977 .

[57]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[58]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[59]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes , 1968, Psychology of Learning and Motivation.