Using visual direction in three-dimensional motion perception

The eyes receive slightly different views of the world, and the differences between their images (binocular disparity) are used to see depth. Several authors have suggested how the brain could exploit this information for three-dimensional (3D) motion perception, but here we consider a simpler strategy. Visual direction is the angle between the direction of an object and the direction that an observer faces. Here we describe human behavioral experiments in which observers use visual direction, rather than binocular information, to estimate an object's 3D motion even though this causes them to make systematic errors. This suggests that recent models of binocular 3D motion perception may not reflect the strategies that human observers actually use.

[1]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  Catching balls: how to get the hand to the right place at the right time. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Accuracy and precision of binocular 3-D motion perception. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  A. Chardenon,et al.  The perceptual control of goal-directed locomotion: a common control architecture for interception and navigation? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  J. M. Foley Binocular distance perception. , 1980, Psychological review.

[5]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[6]  B. G. Cumming,et al.  Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth , 1994, Vision Research.

[7]  D Regan,et al.  The relation between discrimination and sensitivity in the perception of motion in depth. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  G Westheimer,et al.  The spatial sense of the eye. Proctor lecture. , 1979, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[9]  Kevin R Brooks,et al.  Interocular velocity difference contributes to stereomotion speed perception. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[10]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Optic flow and scene structure do not always contribute to the control of human walking , 2002, Vision Research.

[11]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual Guidance of Intercepting a Moving Target on Foot , 2004, Perception.

[12]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Human observers are biased in judging the angular approach of a projectile , 2004, Vision Research.

[13]  John P. Wann,et al.  Why you should look where you are going , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Richard M Wilkie,et al.  Driving as Night Falls The Contribution of Retinal Flow and Visual Direction to the Control of Steering , 2002, Current Biology.

[15]  D. Regan,et al.  Binocular correlates of the direction of motion in depth , 1993, Vision Research.

[16]  J. Wann,et al.  Steering with or without the flow: is the retrieval of heading necessary? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  I. Howard,et al.  Seeing in depth, Vol. 2: Depth perception. , 2002 .

[18]  Leslie Welch,et al.  The precision of size constancy , 1992, Vision Research.