How to not induce SNAs: The insufficiency of directional force
暂无分享,去创建一个
K. | M. Raab | O. Lindemann | M. Fischer | ID A.Michirev
[1] Mark E. Whiting,et al. Beyond Playing 20 Questions with Nature: Integrative Experiment Design in the Social and Behavioral Sciences. , 2022, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[2] M. H. Fischer,et al. Spatial-numerical associations without a motor response? Grip force says 'Yes'. , 2022, Acta psychologica.
[3] M. H. Fischer,et al. Abstract concepts: external influences, internal constraints, and methodological issues , 2022, Psychological Research.
[4] M. Latash,et al. Unintentional force drifts across the human fingers: implications for the neural control of finger tasks , 2022, Experimental Brain Research.
[5] A. Miklashevsky,et al. Catch the star! Spatial information activates the manual motor system , 2021, bioRxiv.
[6] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Advantages masquerading as "issues" in Bayesian hypothesis testing: A commentary on Tendeiro and Kiers (2019). , 2021, Psychological methods.
[7] M. Raab,et al. A Developmental Embodied Choice Perspective Explains the Development of Numerical Choices , 2021, Frontiers in Psychology.
[8] M. H. Fischer,et al. “BreaThink”: breathing affects production and perception of quantities , 2021, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] F. Tomaiuolo,et al. Number space is made by response space: Evidence from left spatial neglect , 2021, Neuropsychologia.
[10] F. Doricchi,et al. How to trigger and keep stable directional Space–Number Associations (SNAs) , 2020, Cortex.
[11] M. H. Fischer,et al. A biological foundation for spatial–numerical associations: the brain's asymmetric frequency tuning , 2020, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[12] K. Moeller,et al. Putting a Finger on Numerical Development – Reviewing the Contributions of Kindergarten Finger Gnosis and Fine Motor Skills to Numerical Abilities , 2020, Frontiers in Psychology.
[13] K. Priftis,et al. Numbers around Descartes: A preregistered study on the three-dimensional SNARC effect , 2019, Cognition.
[14] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Advantages masquerading as "issues" in Bayesian hypothesis testing: A commentary on Tendeiro and Kiers (2019). , 2019, Psychological methods.
[15] Marc Brysbaert,et al. How Many Participants Do We Have to Include in Properly Powered Experiments? A Tutorial of Power Analysis with Reference Tables , 2019, Journal of cognition.
[16] Ulf-Dietrich Reips,et al. The SNARC and MARC effects measured online: Large-scale assessment methods in flexible cognitive effects , 2019, Behavior Research Methods.
[17] M. H. Fischer,et al. Moving arms: the effects of sensorimotor information on the problem-solving process , 2018, Thinking & Reasoning.
[18] H. Nuerk,et al. A large-scale survey on finger counting routines, their temporal stability and flexibility in educated adults , 2018, PeerJ.
[19] Fabrizio Doricchi,et al. Reconstructing the origins of the space-number association: spatial and number-magnitude codes must be used jointly to elicit spatially organised mental number lines , 2018, Cognition.
[20] M. H. Fischer,et al. Deconstructing spatial-numerical associations , 2018, Cognition.
[21] Romain Mathieu,et al. What's Behind a “+” Sign? Perceiving an Arithmetic Operator Recruits Brain Circuits for Spatial Orienting , 2018, Cerebral cortex.
[22] M. Pesenti,et al. Time course of overt attentional shifts in mental arithmetic: Evidence from gaze metrics , 2018, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[23] L. O’Sullivan,et al. Effects of cognitive loading and force on upper trapezius fatigue. , 2017, Occupational medicine.
[24] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. UvA-DARE ( Digital Academic Repository ) Bayesian inference for psychology . Part , 2018 .
[25] Cory D. Bonn,et al. At Birth, Humans Associate “Few” with Left and “Many” with Right , 2017, Current Biology.
[26] F. Simion,et al. A mental number line in human newborns , 2017, bioRxiv.
[27] T. Verguts,et al. The Time Course of Spatial Attention Shifts in Elementary Arithmetic , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[28] Roozbeh Behroozmand,et al. Premotor neural correlates of predictive motor timing for speech production and hand movement: evidence for a temporal predictive code in the motor system , 2017, Experimental Brain Research.
[29] W. Fias,et al. How Does Working Memory Enable Number-Induced Spatial Biases? , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[30] M. Fabbri,et al. Finger counting habit and spatial–numerical association in children and adults , 2016, Consciousness and Cognition.
[31] A. Cangelosi,et al. Ocular drift along the mental number line , 2016, Psychological Research.
[32] R. Proctor,et al. Polarity Correspondence as a General Compatibility Principle , 2015 .
[33] Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al. Gesture as a window onto children’s number knowledge , 2015, Cognition.
[34] Bodo Winter,et al. Mental number space in three dimensions , 2015, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[35] D. Lakens,et al. Can Conceptual Congruency Effects between Number, Time, and Space Be Accounted for by Polarity Correspondence? Acta Psychologica , 2022 .
[36] F. Mast,et al. Spatial biases during mental arithmetic: evidence from eye movements on a blank screen , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[37] M. H. Fischer,et al. Attentional cueing in numerical cognition , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[38] Samuel Shaki,et al. Spatial Associations in Numerical Cognition—From Single Digits to Arithmetic , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[39] Oliver Lindemann,et al. Expyriment: A Python library for cognitive and neuroscientific experiments , 2014, Behavior research methods.
[40] Karenleigh A. Overmann. Finger-counting in the Upper Palaeolithic , 2014 .
[41] Samuel Shaki,et al. Heed the Signs: Operation Signs have Spatial Associations , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[42] Catherine Thevenot,et al. Do not count on me to imagine how I act: behavior contradicts questionnaire responses in the assessment of finger counting habits , 2014, Behavior Research Methods.
[43] K. Moeller,et al. Aspects of situated cognition in embodied numerosity: the case of finger counting , 2014, Cognitive Processing.
[44] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Random walks on the mental number line , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[45] P. König,et al. Where's the action? The pragmatic turn in cognitive science , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[46] Marco Zorzi,et al. The Spatial Representation of Numerical and Non-Numerical Ordered Sequences: Insights from a Random Generation Task , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[47] F. Mast,et al. Moving along the mental number line: Interactions between whole-body motion and numerical cognition. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] Martin H. Fischer,et al. A hierarchical view of grounded, embodied, and situated numerical cognition , 2012, Cognitive Processing.
[49] Carmelo Mario Vicario,et al. Perceiving Numbers Affects the Internal Random Movements Generator , 2012, TheScientificWorldJournal.
[50] M. H. Fischer,et al. Multiple spatial mappings in numerical cognition. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[51] Jan Theeuwes,et al. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.
[52] J. Theeuwes,et al. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.
[53] Martin H. Fischer,et al. When Digits Help Digits: Spatial–Numerical Associations Point to Finger Counting as Prime Example of Embodied Cognition , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[54] Oliver Lindemann,et al. Finger Counting Habits in Middle Eastern and Western Individuals: An Online Survey , 2011 .
[55] Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al. Space, time, and number: a Kantian research program , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[56] Marc Jeannerod,et al. Grip Force Is Part of the Semantic Representation of Manual Action Verbs , 2010, PloS one.
[57] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Understanding in an instant: Neurophysiological evidence for mechanistic language circuits in the brain , 2009, Brain and Language.
[58] Guilherme Wood,et al. On the Cognitive Link between Space and Number: A Meta-Analysis of the SNARC Effect , 2008 .
[59] Giovanna Moretto,et al. Grasping numbers , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[60] Marc Sato,et al. On the relationship between handedness and hand-digit mapping in finger counting , 2008, Cortex.
[61] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations , 2008, Cortex.
[62] T. Loetscher,et al. Head turns bias the brain's internal random generator , 2008, Current Biology.
[63] Harold Bekkering,et al. Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[64] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Moving along the Number Line: Operational Momentum in Nonsymbolic Arithmetic , 2006 .
[65] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. A short tutorial of GPower , 2007 .
[66] T. Loetscher,et al. Exploring number space by random digit generation , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[67] Wim Fias,et al. Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. , 2006, Acta psychologica.
[68] Yang Seok Cho,et al. Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.
[69] Michael Andres,et al. Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction , 2004, Neuroreport.
[70] Takeshi Hatta,et al. Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect , 2004, Memory & cognition.
[71] Wolf Schwarz,et al. Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[72] Vincent Walsh. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[73] Michael D. Dodd,et al. Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[74] Martin H Fischer,et al. Cognitive Representation of Negative Numbers , 2003, Psychological science.
[75] G. Lakoff,et al. Where mathematics comes from : how the embodied mind brings mathematics into being , 2002 .
[76] G. Aschersleben,et al. The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[77] J. Towse,et al. Random Generation of Numbers: A Search for Underlying Processes , 1997 .
[78] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[79] Gary James Jason,et al. The Logic of Scientific Discovery , 1988 .
[80] G. Lakoff,et al. Conceptual Metaphor in Everyday Language , 1980, Shaping Entrepreneurship Research.
[81] Judgment, Decision-Making, and Embodied Choices , 2021 .
[82] Y. Paulignan,et al. A simple technique to study embodied language processes: the grip force sensor , 2017, Behavior research methods.
[83] F. Bremmer,et al. The SNARC effect in two dimensions: Evidence for a frontoparallel mental number plane , 2017, Vision Research.
[84] Ann Dowker,et al. The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition , 2015 .
[85] M. H. Fischer,et al. Random walks on the mental number line , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[86] Bodo Winter,et al. More is up... and right: Random number generation along two axes , 2013, CogSci.
[87] Nils Bender,et al. Das Lateral Preference Inventory: Itemhomogenität der deutschen Version , 2009 .
[88] Roland S. Johansson,et al. Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans , 1996 .
[89] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Space and selective attention , 1994 .
[90] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .