Twisting tongues and memories: Explorations of the relationship between language production and verbal working memory.

Many accounts of working memory posit specialized storage mechanisms for the maintenance of serial order. We explore an alternative, that maintenance is achieved through temporary activation in the language production architecture. Four experiments examined the extent to which the phonological similarity effect can be explained as a sublexical speech error. Phonologically similar nonword stimuli were ordered to create tongue twister or control materials used in four tasks: reading aloud, immediate spoken recall, immediate typed recall, and serial recognition. Dependent measures from working memory (recall accuracy) and language production (speech errors) fields were used. Even though lists were identical except for item order, robust effects of tongue twisters were observed. Speech error analyses showed that errors were better described as phoneme rather than item ordering errors. The distribution of speech errors was comparable across all experiments and exhibited syllable-position effects, suggesting an important role for production processes. Implications for working memory and language production are discussed.

[1]  George Houghton,et al.  Learning artificial phonotactic constraints: time course, durability, and relationship to natural constraints. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  Nomen Leyden studies in linguistics and phonetics , 1969 .

[3]  Gerald Tehan,et al.  Phonological Similarity and Trace Degradation in the Serial Recall Task: When CAT helps RAT, but not MAN , 1999 .

[4]  M. Beckman,et al.  The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[5]  Charles Hulme,et al.  Word-frequency and phonological-neighborhood effects on verbal short-term memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Errors in speech and short-term memory: The effects of phonemic similarity and syllable position , 1980 .

[7]  T. Hartley,et al.  A Linguistically Constrained Model of Short-Term Memory for Nonwords ☆ , 1996 .

[8]  G. Dell,et al.  The sequential cuing effect in speech production , 1994, Cognition.

[9]  Carolyn E. Wilshire,et al.  The “Tongue Twister” Paradigm as a Technique for Studying Phonological Encoding , 1999 .

[10]  M. F. Garrett,et al.  The Analysis of Sentence Production1 , 1975 .

[11]  Chris Mellish,et al.  Current research in natural language generation , 1990 .

[12]  R. Conrad,et al.  Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. , 1964 .

[13]  Prahlad Gupta,et al.  Does Phonological Short-Term Memory Causally Determine Vocabulary Learning? Toward a Computational Resolution of the Debate. , 2009 .

[14]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Exploring the Articulatory Loop , 1984 .

[15]  Sophie K. Scott,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.11 What , 2022 .

[16]  C. Frankish,et al.  Phonotactic influences on short-term memory. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  N. Burgess Memory for Serial Order : A Network Model of the Phonological Loop and its Timing , 1999 .

[18]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  The phonological store of working memory: is it phonological and is it a store? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  S. Shattuck-Hufnagel The role of word structure in segmental serial ordering , 1992, Cognition.

[20]  D. G. MacKay Spoonerisms: the structure of errors in the serial order of speech. , 1970, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  G D Brown,et al.  Oscillator-based memory for serial order. , 2000, Psychological review.

[22]  N. Cowan Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .

[23]  H. H. Clark The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. , 1973 .

[24]  W. A. Wickelgren,et al.  ACOUSTIC SIMILARITY AND INTRUSION ERRORS IN SHORT-TERM MEMORY. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Vocabulary Acquisition and Verbal Short-Term Memory: Computational and Neural Bases , 1997, Brain and Language.

[26]  Victoria A. Fromkin,et al.  The Non-Anomalous Nature of Anomalous Utterances , 1971 .

[27]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Word length and the structure of short-term memory , 1975 .

[28]  R Treiman,et al.  Short-term memory errors for spoken syllables are affected by the linguistic structure of the syllables. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Modeling immediate serial recall with a localist implementation of the primacy model. , 1998 .

[30]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Individual differences in working memory and reading , 1980 .

[31]  Lisa M. Nimmo,et al.  Investigating the Phonological Similarity Effect: Syllable Structure and the Position of Common Phonemes. , 2004 .

[32]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech : Implications for the structure of working memory , 1982 .

[33]  Charles Hulme,et al.  Concrete words are easier to recall than abstract words: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short-term serial recall. , 1999 .

[34]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[35]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study , 1989 .

[36]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[37]  Alan B. Welsh,et al.  Acoustic masking in primary memory. , 1976 .

[38]  W. Cooper,et al.  Sentence Processing: Psycholinguistic Studies Presented to Merrill Garrett. , 1980 .

[39]  A. J. Hull,et al.  INFORMATION, ACOUSTIC CONFUSION AND MEMORY SPAN. , 1964, British journal of psychology.

[40]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[41]  K Bock,et al.  Language production: Methods and methodologies , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[42]  Daniel J. Acheson,et al.  Verbal working memory and language production: Common approaches to the serial ordering of verbal information. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  D. J. Murray Articulation and acoustic confusability in short-term memory. , 1968 .

[44]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  Structure and Content in Language Production: A Theory of Frame Constraints in Phonological Speech Errors , 1993, Cogn. Sci..

[45]  B. Postle Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain , 2006, Neuroscience.

[46]  G. Dell,et al.  Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: a study of the role of experience in language production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Serial Control of Phonology in Speech Production: A Hierarchical Model , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[48]  C. Phillips,et al.  Journal of Memory and Language , 2001 .

[49]  G. Dell,et al.  Language production and serial order: a functional analysis and a model. , 1997, Psychological review.

[50]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span , 1991 .

[51]  P. J. Meijer What Speech Errors Can Tell Us About Word-Form Generation: The Roles of Constraint and Opportunity , 1997 .

[52]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition , 1998 .

[53]  George Houghton,et al.  The problem of serial order: a neural network model of sequence learning and recall , 1990 .

[54]  B. Levy,et al.  Role of articulation in auditory and visual short-term memory , 1971 .

[55]  D. Norris,et al.  The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. , 1998, Psychological review.

[56]  R. Henson Short-Term Memory for Serial Order: The Start-End Model , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[57]  S. Roodenrys,et al.  Sublexical or lexical effects on serial recall of nonwords? , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[58]  Is the Articulatory Loop Articulatory or Auditory? Reexamining the Effects of Concurrent Articulation on Immediate Serial Recall , 1995 .

[59]  G. Dell,et al.  Representation of serial order in speech: evidence from the repeated phoneme effect in speech errors. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[60]  Prahlad Gupta,et al.  Reexamining the phonological similarity effect in immediate serial recall: The roles of type of similarity, category cuing, and item recall , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[61]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Distraction-spanning sustained activity during delayed recognition of locations , 2006, NeuroImage.

[62]  Sieb G. Nooteboom,et al.  The tongue slips into patterns , 1969 .

[63]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Speech errors and the phonological similarity effect in short-term memory: Evidence suggesting a common locus , 2007 .

[64]  G. Dell,et al.  Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. , 1997, Psychological review.

[65]  Satoru Saito,et al.  Irrelevant Sound Disrupts Speech Production: Exploring the Relationship between Short-Term Memory and Experimentally Induced Slips of the Tongue , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[66]  G. Dell,et al.  Models of Impaired Lexical Access in Speech Production , 2000 .

[67]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names , 1990 .