Manipulating cognitive complexity across task types and its impact on learners' interaction during oral performance

Abstract The goal of this study is to investigate the impact of manipulating the cognitive complexity of three different types of oral tasks on interaction. The study first considers the concepts of task complexity and interaction and then examines the specific studies that have looked at the effects of increasing task complexity on conversational interaction. In the experiment, learners of English as a foreign language organized into 27 dyads carry out three different types of tasks: a narrative reconstruction task, an instruction-giving map task, and a decision-making task. Two different versions of each task, one simple and one complex, are presented to learners in different sequences. Task complexity is manipulated along the degree of displaced, past time reference, the number of elements, and the reasoning demands. Audio recordings are transcribed and coded for interactional feedback, which is measured in terms of negotiation of meaning (i.e., confirmation checks, clarification requests, and comprehension checks), recasts, language-related episodes (LREs), and repairs, all of which have been described in the literature as being conducive to acquisition. Both parametric and non-parametric statistical tests are used. Results are discussed in the light of previous studies that have looked at the specific relationship between task complexity and interaction, attention models (Robinson, Applied Linguistics 22: 27–57, 2001a, Second Language Studies 21: 45–105, 2003, International Review of Applied Linguistics 55: 1–32, 2005, International Review of Applied Linguistics 45: 193–213, 2007b; Skehan and Foster, Cognition and tasks, Cambridge University Press, 2001), and how different task types may variously affect the way interaction proceeds during task performance.

[1]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[2]  C D Heth,et al.  Place recognition and way finding by children and adults , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[3]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  A Computational Theory of Complex Problem Solving Using the Vector Space Model ( part II ) : Latent Semantic Analysis Applied to Empirical Results from Adaptation Experiments , 2001 .

[4]  Christopher D Wickens,et al.  Attention to the second language , 2007 .

[5]  Susan M. Gass,et al.  Input, interaction, and the second language learner , 1994 .

[6]  M. Ishida Effects of Recasts on the Acquisition of the Aspectual Form -te i-(ru) by Learners of Japanese as a Foreign Language , 2004 .

[7]  R. Ellis,et al.  Interaction and Second Language Learning : Two Adolescent French Immersion Students Working Together , 2007 .

[8]  Pauline Foster,et al.  Task type and task processing conditions as influences on foreign language performance , 1997 .

[9]  Roger Gilabert Guerrero Task Complexity and L2 Narrative Oral Production , 2005 .

[10]  S. Gass,et al.  Input, Interaction and Output: An Overview , 2006 .

[11]  P. Robinson Chapter 1. Criteria for Classifying and Sequencing Pedagogic Tasks , 2006 .

[12]  ZhaoHong Han A Study of the Impact of Recasts on Tense Consistency in L2 Output , 2002 .

[13]  Alison Mackey,et al.  Responses to Recasts: Repetitions, Primed Production, and Linguistic Development. , 2006 .

[14]  Peter Robinson,et al.  Cognitive Complexity and Task Sequencing: Studies in a Componential Framework for Second Language Task Design , 2005 .

[15]  Roger Gilabert Blanquerna The simultaneous manipulation of task complexity along planning time and + /-Here-and-Now : effects on L 2 oral production , 2005 .

[16]  M. Swain,et al.  Problems in Output and the Cognitive Processes They Generate: A Step Towards Second Language Learning , 1995, Applied Linguistics.

[17]  María del Pilar García Mayo,et al.  Chapter 5. Attention to Form Across Collaborative Tasks by Low-Proficiency Learners in an EFL Setting , 2006 .

[18]  Roger Gilabert Guerrero Effects of manipulating Task Complexity on self-repairs during L2 oral production , 2007 .

[19]  R. Lyster RECASTS, REPETITION, AND AMBIGUITY IN L2 CLASSROOM DISCOURSE , 1998, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[20]  P. Robinson Task complexity, task difficulty, and task production: exploring interactions in a componential framework , 2001 .

[21]  Alison Mackey,et al.  Conversational Interaction and Second Language Development: Recasts, Responses, and Red Herrings? , 1998 .

[22]  Michael H. Long Native Speaker/Non-Native Speaker Conversation in the Second Language Classroom , 1983 .

[23]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .

[24]  Hossein Nassaji Elicitation and Reformulation and Their Relationship with Learner Repair in Dyadic Interaction. , 2007 .

[25]  Teresa Pica,et al.  Chapter 9. Choosing and using communication tasks for second language instruction , 2009 .

[26]  Peter Skehan,et al.  Cognition and Second Language Instruction: Cognition and tasks , 2001 .

[27]  Michael H. Long The Role of the Linguistic Environment in Second Language Acquisition , 1996 .

[28]  M. Swain Three functions of output in second language learning , 1995 .

[29]  R. Schmidt The role of consciousness in second language learning , 1990 .

[30]  H. Wellman,et al.  The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state , 1983, Cognition.

[31]  Alison Mackey,et al.  Interaction research in SLA : A meta-analysis and research synthesis , 2007 .

[32]  S. Gass,et al.  Task-Based Interactions in Classroom and Laboratory Settings. , 2005 .

[33]  David Kortenkamp,et al.  Prototypes, Location, and Associative Networks (PLAN): Towards a Unified Theory of Cognitive Mapping , 1995, Cogn. Sci..

[34]  M. Swain The Output Hypothesis: Theory and Research , 2005 .

[35]  Noriko Iwashita NEGATIVE FEEDBACK AND POSITIVE EVIDENCE IN TASK-BASED INTERACTION , 2003, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[36]  Peter Robinson,et al.  Task Complexity and Second Language Narrative Discourse , 1995 .

[37]  Peter Robinson,et al.  Cognition and Second Language Instruction: Task complexity, cognitive resources, and syllabus design: a triadic framework for examining task influences on SLA , 2001 .

[38]  T. Pica Research on Negotiation: What Does It Reveal about Second-Language Learning Conditions, Processes, and Outcomes?. , 1994 .

[39]  R. Lyster,et al.  CORRECTIVE FEEDBACK AND LEARNER UPTAKE , 1997, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[40]  Peter Robinson,et al.  Task complexity, theory of mind, and intentional reasoning: Effects on L2 speech production, interaction, uptake and perceptions of task difficulty , 2007 .

[41]  Roger Gilabert Chapter 3. The Simultaneous Manipulation of Task Complexity Along Planning Time and (+/- Here-and-Now): Effects on L2 Oral Production , 2006 .

[42]  Leslie Rescorla,et al.  The Use of Psychological State Terms by Late Talkers at Age 3. , 2002 .

[43]  P. Skehan 语言学习认知法 = A cognitive approach to language learning , 1998 .

[44]  Michael H. Long The Least a Second Language Acquisition Theory Needs to Explain , 1990 .

[45]  P. Lightbown,et al.  Recasts as Feedback to Language Learners , 2001 .

[46]  Folkert Kuiken,et al.  The influence of complexity in monologic versus dialogic tasks in Dutch L2 , 2007 .

[47]  Kim Mcdonough,et al.  IDENTIFYING THE IMPACT OF NEGATIVE FEEDBACK AND LEARNERS' RESPONSES ON ESL QUESTION DEVELOPMENT , 2005, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[48]  Merrill Swain,et al.  Focus on form through collaborative dialogue: Exploring task effects , 2001 .

[49]  Andrea Révész Focus on form in task-based language teaching : recasts, task complexity, and L2 learning , 2007 .

[50]  Marta Benítez Fernández-Espartero The use of blogs as international feedback in the efl clasroom: a way to focus on form through conscious reflection , 2011 .

[51]  S. Gass Input and Interaction , 2008 .

[52]  C. Wickens Attention and skilled performance. , 1989 .

[53]  P. Robinson,et al.  Task complexity, the Cognition Hypothesis and second language learning and performance , 2007 .