Distinct and combined responses to environmental geometry and features in a working-memory reorientation task in rats and chicks
暂无分享,去创建一个
Sang Ah Lee | Valeria Anna Sovrano | Giorgio Vallortigara | Anthony McGregor | Colin Lever | Joseph M. Austen
[1] G. Vallortigara,et al. From natural geometry to spatial cognition , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[2] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Two systems of spatial representation underlying navigation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[3] C. Gallistel,et al. The neuroscience of learning: beyond the Hebbian synapse. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.
[4] T. Regier,et al. An adaptive cue combination model of human spatial reorientation , 2017, Cognition.
[5] Laurie L Bloomfield,et al. Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: features overshadow geometry , 2005, Biology Letters.
[6] K. Jeffery,et al. The Boundary Vector Cell Model of Place Cell Firing and Spatial Memory , 2006, Reviews in the neurosciences.
[7] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Navigation by environmental geometry: the use of zebrafish as a model , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[8] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Reorientation and Landmark-Guided Search by Young Children , 2006, Psychological science.
[9] R. C. Honey,et al. Integration of geometric with luminance information in the rat: evidence from within-compound associations. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[10] J. Pearce,et al. Potentiation and overshadowing between landmarks and environmental geometric cues , 2011, Learning & behavior.
[11] D. Olton,et al. Food-searching strategies in young rats: Win-shift predominates over win-stay. , 1978 .
[12] Yutaka Kosaki,et al. Overshadowing of geometry learning by discrete landmarks in the water maze: effects of relative salience and relative validity of competing cues. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[13] Thomas J. Wills,et al. Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry , 2002, Nature.
[14] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Cognitive effects of language on human navigation , 2011, Cognition.
[15] Anthony McGregor,et al. Revaluation of geometric cues reduces landmark discrimination via within-compound associations , 2014, Learning & behavior.
[16] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Independent effects of geometry and landmark in a spontaneous reorientation task: a study of two species of fish , 2012, Animal Cognition.
[17] J. Fodor,et al. The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology , 1984 .
[18] J. Pearce,et al. Between-cue associations influence searching for a hidden goal in an environment with a distinctive shape. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[19] John M. Pearce,et al. Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on cognitive maps but not heading vectors , 1998, Nature.
[20] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: Young children’s use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task , 2012, Cognition.
[21] J. Pearce,et al. Changes in attention to relevant and irrelevant stimuli during spatial learning. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[22] Joseph M. Austen,et al. Dorsolateral striatal lesions impair navigation based on landmark-goal vectors but facilitate spatial learning based on a “cognitive map” , 2015, Learning & memory.
[23] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Spatial Impairment and Memory in Genetic Disorders: Insights from Mouse Models , 2017, Brain sciences.
[24] G. Vallortigara,et al. Hippocampus and medial striatum dissociation during goal navigation by geometry or features in the domestic chick: An immediate early gene study , 2016, Hippocampus.
[25] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Chicks, like children, spontaneously reorient by three-dimensional environmental geometry, not by image matching , 2012, Biology Letters.
[26] J. O’Keefe,et al. Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons , 1996, Nature.
[27] R. L. Fantz. Form preferences in newly hatched chicks. , 1957, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[28] Sang Ah Lee,et al. The boundary-based view of spatial cognition: a synthesis , 2017, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[29] 永福 智志. The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[30] Peter M. Jones,et al. Potentiation, overshadowing, and blocking of spatial learning based on the shape of the environment. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[31] R. Lund,et al. Receptive field properties of single neurons in rat primary visual cortex. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.
[32] Alexandra D. Twyman,et al. Young children's use of features to reorient is more than just associative: further evidence against a modular view of spatial processing. , 2010, Developmental science.
[33] R. Over,et al. Spatial acuity of the chicken , 1981, Brain Research.
[34] M Zanforlin,et al. Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). , 1990, Journal of comparative psychology.
[35] J. O’Keefe,et al. Space in the brain: how the hippocampal formation supports spatial cognition , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[36] J. Fodor. The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .
[37] J. O’Keefe,et al. Modeling place fields in terms of the cortical inputs to the hippocampus , 2000, Hippocampus.
[38] J. O’Keefe,et al. Boundary Vector Cells in the Subiculum of the Hippocampal Formation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[39] J. Pearce,et al. The 36th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: An associative analysis of spatial learning , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[40] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Animals as Natural Geometers , 2009 .
[41] M. Moser,et al. Representation of Geometric Borders in the Entorhinal Cortex , 2008, Science.
[42] Joseph M. Austen,et al. Within-compound associations explain potentiation and failure to overshadow learning based on geometry by discrete landmarks. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[43] Kathryn A Davis,et al. Electrophysiological Signatures of Spatial Boundaries in the Human Subiculum , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[44] M. Cole,et al. Potentiation and overshadowing of shape by wall color in a kite-shaped maze using rats in a foraging task , 2011 .
[45] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Representation of environmental shape in the hippocampus of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus) , 2017, Brain Structure and Function.
[46] Valter Tucci,et al. Working memory and reference memory tests of spatial navigation in mice (Mus musculus). , 2015, Journal of comparative psychology.
[47] Russell A. Epstein,et al. Environmental Geometry Aligns the Hippocampal Map during Spatial Reorientation , 2017, Current Biology.
[48] J. Pearce,et al. A landmark blocks searching for a hidden platform in an environment with a distinctive shape after extended pretraining , 2009, Learning & behavior.
[49] K. Cheng. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.
[50] J. Fodor. The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .
[51] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.
[52] Edward J Golob,et al. Differences between appetitive and aversive reinforcement on reorientation in a spatial working memory task , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.
[53] Rachel Bhushan. Spatial representation from birth to old age: Insights from comparative neurobiology and behavioral genomics. , 2018 .
[54] Thomas J. Wills,et al. Boundary coding in the rat subiculum , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[55] W. Gerstner,et al. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Insights from a rodent navigation model. , 2009, Psychological review.
[56] Christian F. Doeller,et al. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[57] Matthew G. Buckley,et al. Shape Shifting: Local Landmarks Interfere With Navigation by, and Recognition of, Global Shape , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[58] M A Good,et al. Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on the shape of the environment. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.
[59] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Animals' use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size of the experimental space , 2005, Cognition.
[60] Nora S Newcombe,et al. Why size counts: children's spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures. , 2008, Developmental science.
[61] T. Hartley,et al. The Neurobiology of Mammalian Navigation , 2018, Current Biology.
[62] Ken Cheng,et al. 25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: a current theoretical perspective , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[63] Sang Ah Lee,et al. The role of learning and environmental geometry in landmark-based spatial reorientation of fish (Xenotoca eiseni) , 2020, PloS one.
[64] Debbie M. Kelly,et al. Features enhance the encoding of geometry , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[65] J. Taube. The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.