Rhythmic and textural musical sequences differently influence syntax and semantic processing in children.

Effects of music on language processing have been reported separately for syntax and for semantics. Previous studies have shown that regular musical rhythms can facilitate syntax processing and that semantic features of musical excerpts can influence semantic processing of words. It remains unclear whether musical parameters, such as rhythm and sound texture, may specifically influence different components of linguistic processing. In the current study, two types of musical sequences (one focusing on rhythm and the other focusing on sound texture) were presented to children who were requested to perform a syntax or a semantic task thereafter. The results revealed that rhythmic and textural musical sequences differently influence syntax and semantic processing. For grammaticality judgments, children's performance was better after regular rhythmic sequences than after textural sound sequences. In the semantic evocation task, children produced more numerous and more various concepts after textural sound sequences than after regular rhythmic sequences. These results suggest that rhythm boosts perceptual and cognitive sequencing required in syntax processing, whereas texture promote verbalization and concept activation in verbal production. The findings have implications for the interpretation of musical priming effects and are discussed in the frameworks of dynamic attending and conceptual processing.

[1]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Exposure to music and cognitive performance: tests of children and adults , 2007 .

[2]  Boris New,et al.  Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE , 2001 .

[3]  James A. Anderson,et al.  The BSB model: a simple nonlinear autoassociative neural network , 1993 .

[4]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children , 2009, NeuroImage.

[5]  J. Devin McAuley,et al.  Perspectives on the rhythm–grammar link and its implications for typical and atypical language development , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Bernard Lété,et al.  MANULEX: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary school readers , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[7]  Susan Hallam,et al.  Music Listening and Cognitive Abilities in 10‐ and 11‐Year‐Olds: The Blur Effect , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[8]  S. Kotz,et al.  Cortical speech processing unplugged: a timely subcortico-cortical framework , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  Reyna L. Gordon,et al.  New evidence of a rhythmic priming effect that enhances grammaticality judgments in children , 2017, bioRxiv.

[10]  L. Trainor,et al.  Music acquisition: effects of enculturation and formal training on development , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Pierre Schaeffer Traité des objets musicaux , 1966 .

[12]  P. Cayé-Thomasen,et al.  Language understanding and vocabulary of early cochlear implanted children. , 2013, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.

[13]  Jérôme Daltrozzo,et al.  Is conceptual processing in music automatic? An electrophysiological approach , 2009, Brain Research.

[14]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. , 1976, Psychological review.

[15]  Robert F. Port,et al.  Meter and speech , 2003, J. Phonetics.

[16]  David Poeppel,et al.  The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral lateralization as 'asymmetric sampling in time' , 2003, Speech Commun..

[17]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Short-Term Music Training Enhances Verbal Intelligence and Executive Function , 2011, Psychological science.

[18]  James L. Morgan,et al.  Signal to syntax : bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition , 1996 .

[19]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Arousal, Mood, and The Mozart Effect , 2001, Psychological science.

[20]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[21]  T. McNamara Semantic Priming: Perspectives from Memory and Word Recognition , 2005 .

[22]  S. Koelsch Towards a neural basis of processing musical semantics. , 2011, Physics of life reviews.

[23]  Jérôme Daltrozzo,et al.  Conceptual Processing in Music as Revealed by N400 Effects on Words and Musical Targets , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  E. Large,et al.  Neural Networks for Beat Perception in Musical Rhythm , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[25]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Effects of Musical Tempo and Mode on Arousal, Mood, and Spatial Abilities , 2002 .

[26]  Sonja A. Kotz,et al.  Entrainment of syntactic processing? ERP-responses to predictable time intervals during syntactic reanalysis , 2008, Brain Research.

[27]  Helmut Schmidt,et al.  Probabilistic part-of-speech tagging using decision trees , 1994 .

[28]  M. Jones,et al.  Temporal Aspects of Stimulus-Driven Attending in Dynamic Arrays , 2002, Psychological science.

[29]  Manuela M Marin Effects of Early Musical Training on Musical and Linguistic Syntactic Abilities , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[30]  J. Snyder,et al.  Pulse and Meter as Neural Resonance , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Richard Kronland-Martinet,et al.  The Evocative Power of Sounds: Conceptual Priming between Words and Nonverbal Sounds , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Barbara Tillmann,et al.  Temporally Regular Musical Primes Facilitate Subsequent Syntax Processing in Children with Specific Language Impairment , 2016, Front. Neurosci..

[33]  Yufang Yang,et al.  Spatial conceptual associations between music and pictures as revealed by N400 effect. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[34]  Doris Eckstein,et al.  Musical Chords as Affective Priming Context in a Word-Evaluation Task , 2003 .

[35]  N Bedoin,et al.  Boosting syntax training with temporally regular musical primes in children with cochlear implants. , 2017, Annals of physical and rehabilitation medicine.

[36]  A. Friederici,et al.  Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Dynamic attending and responses to time. , 1989, Psychological review.

[38]  E. Bates,et al.  Picture Naming and Lexical Access in Italian Children and Adults , 2001 .

[39]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[40]  Barbara Tillmann,et al.  Rhythmic auditory stimulation influences syntactic processing in children with developmental language disorders. , 2013, Neuropsychology.

[41]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Affective Priming Effects of Musical Sounds on the Processing of Word Meaning , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Music and Cognitive Abilities , 2005 .

[43]  J. Yordanova,et al.  Is there a specific Vivaldi effect on verbal memory functions? Evidence from listening to music in younger and older adults , 2019 .

[44]  Hugo Quené,et al.  Effects of Timing Regularity and Metrical Expectancy on Spoken-Word Perception , 2005, Phonetica.

[45]  Karli M Nave,et al.  The Developmental Origins of the Perception and Production of Musical Rhythm , 2018 .

[46]  B. Tillmann,et al.  Rhythmic priming of grammaticality judgments in children: Duration matters. , 2020, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[47]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Can out-of-context musical sounds convey meaning? An ERP study on the processing of meaning in music. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[48]  Yufang Yang,et al.  Conveying the concept of movement in music: An event-related brain potential study , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Serge Heiden,et al.  The TXM Platform: Building Open-Source Textual Analysis Software Compatible with the TEI Encoding Scheme , 2010, PACLIC.

[50]  F. Rauscher,et al.  Music and spatial task performance , 1993, Nature.

[51]  Anne Christophe,et al.  Phrasal prosody constrains syntactic analysis in toddlers , 2017, Cognition.

[52]  Aniruddh D. Patel Music, Language, and the Brain , 2007 .

[53]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Music playschool enhances children’s linguistic skills , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[54]  A. Villringer,et al.  Musical meaning modulates word acquisition , 2019, Brain and Language.

[55]  G. Schlaug,et al.  Practicing a Musical Instrument in Childhood is Associated with Enhanced Verbal Ability and Nonverbal Reasoning , 2008, PloS one.

[56]  Nina C Capone,et al.  Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[57]  David Poeppel,et al.  Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[58]  S. Koelsch,et al.  Supplementary Information for Effects of Sad and Happy Music on Mind-Wandering and the Default Mode Network , 2017 .

[59]  J. Devin McAuley,et al.  Musical rhythm discrimination explains individual differences in grammar skills in children. , 2015, Developmental science.