The Neurobiology of Rhyme Judgment by Deaf and Hearing Adults: An ERP Study

We used electrophysiology to determine the time course and distribution of neural activation during an English word rhyme task in hearing and congenitally deaf adults. Behavioral performance by hearing participants was at ceiling and their ERP data replicated two robust effects repeatedly observed in the literature. First, a sustained negativity, termed the contingent negative variation, was elicited following the first stimulus word. This negativity was asymmetric, being more negative over the left than right sites. The second effect we replicated in hearing participants was an enhanced negativity (N450) to nonrhyming second stimulus words. This was largest over medial, parietal regions of the right hemisphere. Accuracy on the rhyme task by the deaf group as a whole was above chance level, yet significantly poorer than hearing participants. We examined only ERP data from deaf participants who performed the task above chance level (n = 9). We observed indications of subtle differences in ERP responses between deaf and hearing groups. However, overall the patterns in the deaf group were very similar to that in the hearing group. Deaf participants, just as hearing participants, showed greater negativity to nonrhyming than rhyming words. Furthermore the onset latency of this effect was the same as that observed in hearing participants. Overall, the neural processes supporting explicit phonological judgments are very similar in deaf and hearing people, despite differences in the modality of spoken language experience. This supports the suggestion that phonological processing is to a large degree amodal or supramodal.

[1]  Mairéad MacSweeney,et al.  Predictors of reading delay in deaf adolescents: the relative contributions of rapid automatized naming speed and phonological awareness and decoding. , 2003, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.

[2]  Asaid Khateb,et al.  Rhyme processing in the brain: an ERP mapping study. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[3]  Bencie Woll,et al.  Phonological processing in deaf signers and the impact of age of first language acquisition , 2008, NeuroImage.

[4]  J. Leybaert,et al.  The Rhyming Skills of Deaf Children Educated with Phonetically Augmented Speechreading , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[5]  M. Rugg,et al.  Asymmetries in event-related potentials during rhyme-matching: Confirmation of the null effects of handedness , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  A. Grabowska,et al.  Implicit phonological and semantic processing in children with developmental dyslexia: Evidence from event-related potentials , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  D. Bors,et al.  Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices: Norms for First-Year University Students and the Development of a Short Form , 1998 .

[8]  R. Dykman,et al.  Visual event-related potentials of dyslexic children to rhyming and nonrhyming stimuli. , 1994, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[9]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and the phonological processing of words and non-words , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials in phonological matching tasks , 1984, Brain and Language.

[11]  Ron Dumont,et al.  Gates-Macginitie Reading Test , 2008 .

[12]  J. Leybaert,et al.  Lateralization effects during semantic and rhyme judgement tasks in deaf and hearing subjects , 2003, Brain and Language.

[13]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[14]  M A Bobes,et al.  Brain potentials and the availability of semantic and phonological codes over time , 1994, Neuroreport.

[15]  Michael J. Brammer,et al.  Enhanced activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in deaf and dyslexic adults during rhyming , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  U. Goswami,et al.  Phonological awareness of syllables, rhymes, and phonemes in deaf children. , 2000, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[17]  M. Kutas,et al.  The lateral distribution of event-related potentials during sentence processing , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Donna Coch,et al.  Three kinds of rhymes: An ERP study , 2008, Brain and Language.

[19]  A. Milner,et al.  Visual evoked potentials to lateralized visual stimuli and the measurement of interhemispheric transmission time , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  U. Goswami,et al.  Orthographic influences, vocabulary development, and phonological awareness in deaf children who use cochlear implants , 2009, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[21]  R. Campbell,et al.  Deafness, Spelling and Rhyme: How Spelling Supports Written Word and Picture Rhyming Skills in Deaf Subjects , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[22]  Donna Coch,et al.  The case of letter rhyming: an ERP study. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[23]  Colin J Davis,et al.  The spatial coding model of visual word identification. , 2010, Psychological review.

[24]  David I. Perrett,et al.  Event-related potentials and the matching of familiar and unfamiliar faces , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  M. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures , 1990, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  R. Spencer,et al.  Phonologic processing in adults who stutter: electrophysiological and behavioral evidence. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[27]  P. Holcomb,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Elicited during Phonological Processing Differentiate Subgroups of Reading Disabled Adolescents , 1998, Brain and Language.

[28]  Marie-Noëlle Metz-Lutz,et al.  Phonological processing in relation to reading: An fMRI study in deaf readers , 2007, NeuroImage.

[29]  Phillip J. Holcomb,et al.  Phonological Processing in Visual Rhyming: A Developmental ERP Study , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Christine Weber-Fox,et al.  Development of neural processes mediating rhyme judgments: Phonological and orthographic interactions. , 2003, Developmental psychobiology.

[31]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and the interaction between orthographic and phonological information in a rhyme-judgment task , 1987, Brain and Language.

[32]  U. Frith Cognitive Processes in Spelling , 1980 .

[33]  T. Münte,et al.  Semantic, syntactic, and phonological processing of written words in adult developmental dyslexic readers: an event-related brain potential study , 2007, BMC Neuroscience.

[34]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[35]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words , 1980, Biological Psychology.

[36]  J. Leybaert Phonological representations in deaf children: the importance of early linguistic experience. , 1998, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[37]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Max Coltheart,et al.  The MRC Psycholinguistic Database , 1981 .

[39]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[40]  W. Walter,et al.  Contingent Negative Variation : An Electric Sign of Sensori-Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain , 1964, Nature.

[41]  Amy M. Lieberman,et al.  Achievement in Relation to Phonological Coding and Awareness in Deaf Readers : A Meta-analysis , 2011 .

[42]  Jette G. Hansen Edwards,et al.  Phonology and second language acquisition , 2008 .