Object-based apparent motion

The interpretation of a dynamic visual scene requires integrating information within frames (grouping and completion) and across frames (correspondence matching). Fragmentary views of objects were used in five experiments. These views could not be matched with each other by any similarity transformation on the basis of their explicit visual features, but their completed versions were related by a rotational transformation. When the fragmentary images were successively presented to observers, it was found that they produced apparent motion in the picture plane and in depth. Thus, apparent motion is capable of establishing correspondence at the level of perceptually recovered objects in three-dimensional space.

[1]  Kavitha Srinivas Perceptual specificity in nonverbal priming. , 1993 .

[2]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Apparent movement with subjective contours. , 1973, Vision research.

[3]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Objects, Attributes, and Visual Attention: Which, What, and Where , 1992 .

[4]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion: the long and short of it. , 1989, Spatial vision.

[5]  Ennio Mingolla,et al.  Neural dynamics of perceptual grouping: Textures, boundaries, and emergent segmentations , 1985 .

[6]  A Koriat,et al.  Establishing global and local correspondence between successive stimuli: the holistic nature of backward alignment. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  P A Kolers,et al.  Figural change in apparent motion. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  S Ullman,et al.  Two Dimensionality of the Correspondence Process in Apparent Motion , 1978, Perception.

[9]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Images and Their Transformations , 1982 .

[10]  K Nakayama,et al.  Stereoscopic Depth: Its Relation to Image Segmentation, Grouping, and the Recognition of Occluded Objects , 1989, Perception.

[11]  C. Bundesen,et al.  Velocity Constraints on Apparent Rotational Movement , 1982, Perception.

[12]  F. Attneave,et al.  Apparent movement in tridimensional space , 1973 .

[13]  I. Biederman,et al.  Size invariance in visual object priming , 1992 .

[14]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Figure-ground segregation modulates apparent motion , 1986, Vision Research.

[15]  H. H. Corbin The perception of grouping and apparent movement in visual depth. , 1942 .

[16]  I. Biederman,et al.  Priming contour-deleted images: Evidence for intermediate representations in visual object recognition , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  R N Shepard,et al.  Shape, orientation, and apparent rotational motion. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  C Bundesen,et al.  Visual Apparent Movement: Transformations of Size and Orientation , 1983, Perception.

[19]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  M Green,et al.  Correspondence matching in apparent motion: evidence for three-dimensional spatial representation. , 1986, Science.

[21]  I M Smith,et al.  The Effect of Two-Dimensional and Three-Dimensional Distance on Apparent Motion , 1983, Perception.

[22]  William H. Warren,et al.  Visual information for object identity in apparent movement , 1977 .

[23]  R N Shepard,et al.  Apparent motion between shapes differing in location and orientation: A window technique for estimating path curvature , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  R. Shepard,et al.  Perceptual illusion of rotation of three-dimensional objects. , 1976, Science.

[25]  P. Kellman,et al.  A theory of visual interpolation in object perception , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  V S Ramachandran,et al.  Kinetic Occlusion by Apparent Movement , 1985, Perception.

[27]  V. Ramachandran Vision: Interactions between motion, depth, color and form: the utilitarian theory of perception , 1991 .

[28]  J. Norman,et al.  Recognition of rotated letters: extracting invariance across successive and simultaneous stimuli. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Frames of reference for allocating attention to space: Evidence from the neglect syndrome , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  A Koriat,et al.  Frames and images: sequential effects in mental rotation. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Preattentive recovery of three-dimensional orientation from line drawings. , 1991, Psychological review.

[32]  J. Farrell,et al.  Visual transformations underlying apparent movement , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  G. Kanizsa,et al.  Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Perception , 1979 .

[34]  L. Brooks,et al.  Episodic effects on picture identification: implications for theories of concept learning and theories of memory. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  Petersik Jt The two-process distinction in apparent motion. , 1989 .

[36]  R. Shepard Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.

[37]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[38]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  On the perception of shape from shading , 1988, Nature.

[39]  S. Ullman,et al.  The interpretation of visual motion , 1977 .

[40]  R. Leeper A Study of a Neglected Portion of the Field of Learning—the Development of Sensory Organization , 1935 .

[41]  R E Shaw,et al.  The visual specification of events: A response to Ullman , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  S. M. Axstis PHI MOVEMENT AS A SUBTRACTION PROCESS , 1970 .

[43]  Stephen E. Palmer,et al.  Perception of partly occluded objects: A microgenetic analysis. , 1992 .

[44]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion , 1989, Vision Research.

[45]  G. Mather Temporal Properties of Apparent Motion in Subjective Figures , 1988, Perception.

[46]  I. Biederman,et al.  Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. , 1992, Psychological review.

[47]  A. Kramer,et al.  Perceptual organization and focused attention: The role of objects and proximity in visual processing , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[48]  I. Biederman,et al.  Evidence for Complete Translational and Reflectional Invariance in Visual Object Priming , 1991, Perception.

[49]  O J Braddick,et al.  Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[50]  J. Freyd,et al.  Apparent Motion of the Human Body , 1990 .

[51]  P. A. Kolers Aspects of motion perception , 1972 .

[52]  G. Baylis,et al.  Visual attention and objects: evidence for hierarchical coding of location. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[53]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Amodal Representation of Occluded Surfaces: Role of Invisible Stimuli in Apparent Motion Correspondence , 1990, Perception.

[54]  Mary M. Conte,et al.  Motion mechanisms have only limited access to form information , 1990, Vision Research.

[55]  J. B. Pittenger,et al.  Comment on Warren’s Visual information for object identity in apparent movement , 1977 .

[56]  O. Reiser,et al.  Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .

[57]  I Biederman,et al.  Metric invariance in object recognition: a review and further evidence. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[58]  S. Tipper,et al.  Short Report: Object-Centred Inhibition of Return of Visual Attention , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[59]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Priming effects in picture fragment completion: support for the perceptual closure hypothesis. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. General.