Review of semantic cognition : A parallel distributed processing approach

In this précis of our recent book, Semantic Cognition: A Parallel Distributed Processing Approach (Rogers & McClelland 2004), we present a parallel distributed processing theory of the acquisition, representation, and use of human semantic knowledge. The theory proposes that semantic abilities arise from the flow of activation among simple, neuron-like processing units, as governed by the strengths of interconnecting weights; and that acquisition of new semantic information involves the gradual adjustment of weights in the system in response to experience. These simple ideas explain a wide range of empirical phenomena from studies of categorization, lexical acquisition, and disordered semantic cognition. In this précis we focus on phenomena central to the reaction against similarity-based theories that arose in the 1980s and that subsequently motivated the “theory-theory” approach to semantic knowledge. Specifically, we consider (1) how concepts differentiate in early development, (2) why some groupings of items seem to form “good” or coherent categories while others do not, (3) why different properties seem central or important to different concepts, (4) why children and adults sometimes attest to beliefs that seem to contradict their direct experience, (5) how concepts reorganize between the ages of 4 and 10, and (6) the relationship between causal knowledge and semantic knowledge. The explanations our theory offers for these phenomena are illustrated with reference to a simple feedforward connectionist model. The relationships between this simple model, the broader theory, and more general issues in cognitive science are discussed.

[1]  Linda B. Smith From Knowledge to Knowing: Real Progress in the Study of Infant Categorization. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[2]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Theory-Based Induction , 2003 .

[3]  U. Neisser Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization , 1989 .

[4]  James L. McClelland,et al.  How Language Affects Thought in a Connectionist Model , 2007 .

[5]  Mark F. St. John,et al.  The Story Gestalt: A Model of Knowledge-Intensive Processes in Text Comprehension , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[6]  T. Rogers,et al.  Neural basis of category-specific semantic deficits for living things: evidence from semantic dementia, HSVE and a neural network model. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Semantic Cognition: A Parallel Distributed Processing Approach , 2004 .

[8]  P. Johnson-Laird How We Reason , 2006 .

[9]  J. Fodor,et al.  Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.

[10]  James L. McClelland Parallel Distributed Processing: Implications for Cognition and Development , 1988 .

[11]  Alex Martin,et al.  Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  E. Rosch Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories. , 1975 .

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Learning and Applying Contextual Constraints in Sentence Comprehension , 1990, Artif. Intell..

[14]  A WilsonRobert,et al.  The Shadows and Shallows of Explanation , 1998 .

[15]  B. Malt,et al.  Category Coherence in Cross-Cultural Perspective , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  E. Spelke,et al.  Origins of knowledge. , 1992, Psychological review.

[17]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Attractor Dynamics in the Hippocampal Representation of the Local Environment , 2005, Science.

[18]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A computational model of semantic memory impairment: modality specificity and emergent category specificity. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  John E. Opfer,et al.  Identifying living and sentient kinds from dynamic information: the case of goal-directed versus aimless autonomous movement in conceptual change , 2002, Cognition.

[20]  Douglas L. T. Rohde,et al.  Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small? , 1999, Cognition.

[21]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Sensitivity to Sampling in Bayesian Word Learning We Thank Members of the Ubc Baby Cognition Lab for Their Help with Data Collection, And , 2022 .

[22]  Emilie M. Roth,et al.  The effect of context on the structure of categories , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  Gerry Altmann,et al.  Mapping across Domains Without Feedback: A Neural Network Model of Transfer of Implicit Knowledge , 1999, Cogn. Sci..

[24]  S. Waxman,et al.  Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  G. Marcus The Algebraic Mind: Integrating Connectionism and Cognitive Science , 2001 .

[26]  Rajat Raina,et al.  Self-taught learning: transfer learning from unlabeled data , 2007, ICML '07.

[27]  W. Ahn Why are different features central for natural kinds and artifacts?: the role of causal status in determining feature centrality , 1998, Cognition.

[28]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[29]  John R. Anderson The Adaptive Character of Thought , 1990 .

[30]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Levels indeed! A response to Broadbent , 1985 .

[31]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The Morton-Massaro law of information integration: implications for models of perception. , 2001, Psychological review.

[32]  E. R. Siqueland,et al.  The nature and structure of infant form categories , 1983 .

[33]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Did, Made, Had, Said: Capturing Quasi-Regularity in Exception , 2003 .

[34]  Menno van Zaanen Bootstrapping structure using similarity , 1999, CLIN.

[35]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Linguistic Adaptation without Linguistic Constraints: The Role of Sequential Learning in Language Evolution , 2004 .

[36]  Paul Smolensky,et al.  Information processing in dynamical systems: foundations of harmony theory , 1986 .

[37]  B. Rehder A causal-model theory of conceptual representation and categorization. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[38]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[39]  T. M. Mayhew,et al.  Anatomy of the Cortex: Statistics and Geometry. , 1991 .

[40]  J. Fodor,et al.  Connectionism and the problem of systematicity: Why Smolensky's solution doesn't work , 1990, Cognition.

[41]  Grover Hudson,et al.  PHONOLOGY AND LANGUAGE USE , 2004 .

[42]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed model of human learning and memory , 1986 .

[43]  S. E. Robbins Direct Memory On the Possibility of Direct Memory , 2006 .

[44]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[45]  H. Wellman,et al.  Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious , 1991, Cognition.

[46]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[47]  K. Holyoak,et al.  A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization. , 2003, Psychological review.

[48]  J. Mandler Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Infancy , 2000 .

[49]  R. Siegler,et al.  Developmental Differences in Rule Learning: A Microgenetic Analysis , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[50]  H. Chertkow,et al.  Semantic memory , 2002, Current neurology and neuroscience reports.

[51]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The Adaptive Nature of Human Categorization , 1991 .

[52]  S. Pinker,et al.  On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition , 1988, Cognition.

[53]  David J. C. MacKay,et al.  A Practical Bayesian Framework for Backpropagation Networks , 1992, Neural Computation.

[54]  M. Tarr,et al.  Unraveling mechanisms for expert object recognition: bridging brain activity and behavior. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  G. J. Savelsbergh,et al.  Grasping tau. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  Menno van Zaanen Implementing Alignment-Based Learning , 2002, ICGI.

[57]  Massimo Marraffa The Mind Doesn't Work That Way. The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology , 2001 .

[58]  Robert F. Hadley Cognition and the computational power of connectionist networks , 2000, Connect. Sci..

[59]  Silvia P. Gennari,et al.  Knowing versus Naming: Similarity and the Linguistic Categorization of Artifacts , 1999 .

[60]  P. Quinn Is the asymmetry in young infants’ categorization of humans versus nonhuman animals based on head, body, or global gestalt information? , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[61]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Revisiting preschoolers’ living things concept: A microgenetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[62]  D. Medin,et al.  A relevance theory of induction , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[63]  John R. Anderson,et al.  A hybrid model of categorization , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[64]  J. Tanaka,et al.  Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[65]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[66]  Alessandro Treves,et al.  Uninformative memories will prevail: The storage of correlated representations and its consequences , 2007, HFSP journal.

[67]  Samy Bengio,et al.  Taking on the curse of dimensionality in joint distributions using neural networks , 2000, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks Learn. Syst..

[68]  Marshall R. Mayberry,et al.  A Connectionist Model of Sentence Comprehension in Visual Worlds , 2005 .

[69]  S. Sloman Feature-Based Induction , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[70]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[71]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  CARVING NATURE AT ITS JOINTS AND CARVING JOINTS INTO NATURE: HOW LABELS AUGMENT CATEGORY REPRESENTATIONS , 2005 .

[72]  Alice G. B. ter Meulen Dynamic definite descriptions, implicit arguments and familiarity , 2004 .

[73]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Reducing the Dimensionality of Data with Neural Networks , 2006, Science.

[74]  J. Tanaka The entry point of face recognition: evidence for face expertise. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[75]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Learning the structure of event sequences. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[76]  Gary F. Marcus,et al.  Children's overregularization and its implications for cognition , 2000 .

[77]  David E. Rumelhart,et al.  Brain style computation: learning and generalization , 1990 .

[78]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Connectionist models of development , 2003 .

[79]  D. Medin,et al.  SUSTAIN: a network model of category learning. , 2004, Psychological review.

[80]  H Pashler,et al.  How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing. , 2000, Psychological review.

[81]  T. Griffiths,et al.  Probabilistic inference in human semantic memory , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[82]  I. Mechelen,et al.  Categories and Concepts · Theoretical Views and Inductive Data Analysis , 2009 .

[83]  F. Keil Mapping the mind: The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains: The origins of concepts of living things , 1994 .

[84]  T. Rogers,et al.  Object categorization: reversals and explanations of the basic-level advantage. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[85]  Ian Dennis,et al.  : Working memory, inhibitory control and the development of children's reasoning , 2004 .

[86]  R. Brown How shall a thing be called. , 1958, Psychological review.

[87]  S. Pauen The global-to-basic level shift in infants’ categorical thinking: First evidence from a longitudinal study , 2002 .

[88]  S. Pauen Evidence for knowledge-based category discrimination in infancy. , 2002, Child development.

[89]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Learning and using relational theories , 2007, NIPS.

[90]  R. Gelman,et al.  Preschooler's Ability to Decide Whether a Photographed Unfamiliar Object Can Move Itself. , 1988 .

[91]  Thomas R. Schultz,et al.  A Connectionist Model of the Development of Transitivity , 2004 .

[92]  N. Chater,et al.  Rational models of cognition , 1998 .

[93]  Refractor Vision , 2000, The Lancet.

[94]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Children's development of analogical reasoning: insights from scene analogy problems. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[95]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  An Introduction to Neural and Electronic Networks , 1995 .

[96]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Word learning as Bayesian inference. , 2007, Psychological review.

[97]  Megan J. Bulloch,et al.  Causal relations drive young children’s induction, naming, and categorization , 2007, Cognition.

[98]  David H. Sharp,et al.  A connectionist model of development. , 1991, Journal of theoretical biology.

[99]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Mechanisms of Implicit Learning: Connectionist Models of Sequence Processing , 1993 .

[100]  David M. Sobel,et al.  Detecting blickets: how young children use information about novel causal powers in categorization and induction. , 2000, Child development.

[101]  R. Horton Rules and representations , 1993, The Lancet.

[102]  A. Gopnik,et al.  The theory theory. , 1994 .

[103]  Francis Jeffry Pelletier,et al.  The Generic book , 1997 .

[104]  James L. McClelland Stochastic Interactive Activation and the Effect of Context on Perception , 1989 .

[105]  D. Medin,et al.  The role of theories in conceptual coherence. , 1985, Psychological review.

[106]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Perceptual Similarity and Conceptual Structure , 1992 .

[107]  James A. Hampton,et al.  The inverse conjunction fallacy , 2006 .

[108]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Combining causal and similarity-based reasoning , 2006, NIPS.

[109]  J. Elman Distributed representations, simple recurrent networks, and grammatical structure , 1991, Machine Learning.

[110]  Leonidas A A Doumas,et al.  A theory of the discovery and predication of relational concepts. , 2008, Psychological review.

[111]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Mechanisms of Analogical Learning. , 1987 .

[112]  A. Feeney How many processes underlie category-based induction? Effects of conclusion specificity and cognitive ability , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[113]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Sentence comprehension: A parallel distributed processing approach , 1989, Language and Cognitive Processes.

[114]  Michael B. Lewis,et al.  Capgras delusion: a window on face recognition , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[115]  Antonio R. Damasio,et al.  The Brain Binds Entities and Events by Multiregional Activation from Convergence Zones , 1989, Neural Computation.

[116]  Hadyn D. Ellis,et al.  Reduced autonomic responses to faces in Capgras delusion , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[117]  Warren S. Sarle,et al.  Neural Networks and Statistical Models , 1994 .

[118]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning distributed representations of concepts. , 1989 .

[119]  Alessandro Treves,et al.  Frontal latching networks: a possible neural basis for infinite recursion , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[120]  David C. Plaut,et al.  A connectionist model of sentence comprehension and production , 2002 .

[121]  Liliana Albertazzi,et al.  Psychology from an empirical standpoint , 2006 .

[122]  P. Kay,et al.  Resolving the question of color naming universals , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[123]  S. Sloman The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .

[124]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Global-Before-Basic Object Categorization in Connectionist Networks and 2-Month-Old Infants. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[125]  S. E. Robbins Bergson and the holographic theory of mind , 2006 .

[126]  David C. Plaut,et al.  Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .

[127]  James Woodward,et al.  Law and Explanation in Biology: Invariance Is the Kind of Stability That Matters , 2001, Philosophy of Science.

[128]  E. Markman,et al.  Categories and induction in young children , 1986, Cognition.

[129]  J. Mandler,et al.  Concept formation in infancy , 1993 .

[130]  Agb ter Meulen,et al.  Chronoscopes - The dynamic representation of facts and events , 2000 .

[131]  Frank C. Keil,et al.  The Shadows and Shallows of Explanation , 1998, Minds and Machines.

[132]  C. Frith The pathology of experience. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[133]  G. Kane Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, vol 1: Foundations, vol 2: Psychological and Biological Models , 1994 .

[134]  Peter M. Todd,et al.  Learning and connectionist representations , 1993 .

[135]  Yves Chauvin,et al.  Backpropagation: the basic theory , 1995 .

[136]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Learning Causal Schemata , 2007 .

[137]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Studies on the formation of perceptually based basic-level categories in young infants. , 1994, Child development.

[138]  S. Levinson Cut and break verbs in Yélî Dnye, the Papuan language of Rossel Island , 2007 .

[139]  J. Mandler,et al.  Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[140]  J. Mandler How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representational system☆ , 1988 .

[141]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition , 1993 .

[142]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[143]  A. Riegler,et al.  When Coffee Cups Are Like Old Elephants or Why Representation Modules Don ’ t Make Sense , 1997 .

[144]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[145]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Activation of the Human Superior Temporal Gyrus during Observation of Goal Attribution by Intentional Objects , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[146]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Words as Invitations to Form Categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-Month-Old Infants , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[147]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[148]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  OPTIMAL PERCEPTUAL INFERENCE , 1983 .

[149]  John Garcia,et al.  Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning , 1966 .

[150]  J. F. Macario,et al.  Young children's use of color in classification: Foods and canonically colored objects , 1991 .

[151]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Mental Leaps: Analogy in Creative Thought , 1994 .

[152]  J. Hampton The verification of category and property statements , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[153]  E. Spelke,et al.  Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change , 1994 .

[154]  Fei Xu The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy , 2002, Cognition.

[155]  S. Laurence,et al.  Concepts and Cognitive Science , 1999 .

[156]  Marc'Aurelio Ranzato,et al.  Efficient Learning of Sparse Representations with an Energy-Based Model , 2006, NIPS.

[157]  L. Barsalou Situated simulation in the human conceptual system , 2003 .

[158]  Emilio Kropff Full solution for the storage of correlated memories in an autoassociative memory , 2007 .

[159]  D. Broadbent A question of levels: comment on McClelland and Rumelhart , 1985 .

[160]  Stephen E. Robbins,et al.  Semantics, experience and time , 2002, Cognitive Systems Research.

[161]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  A voxel‐based morphometry study of semantic dementia: Relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory , 2000, Annals of neurology.

[162]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning representations by back-propagating errors , 1986, Nature.

[163]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Rules and Similarity in Concept Learning , 1999, NIPS.

[164]  L. Rips The Psychology of Proof , 1994 .

[165]  Yoshua Bengio,et al.  Greedy Layer-Wise Training of Deep Networks , 2006, NIPS.

[166]  Verena D. Schmittmann,et al.  Multiple Learning Modes in the Development of Rule-Based Category-learning Task Performance , 2005 .

[167]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[168]  Chris McNorgan,et al.  An attractor model of lexical conceptual processing: simulating semantic priming , 1999, Cogn. Sci..

[169]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Structure-Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy , 1983, Cogn. Sci..

[170]  Lawrence W. Barsalou,et al.  The instability of graded structure: implications for the nature of concepts , 1987 .

[171]  L B Cohen,et al.  Developmental change in infants' perception of correlations among attributes. , 1986, Child development.

[172]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[173]  Zachary Estes,et al.  Metamorphosis : Essence , appearance , and behavior in the categorization of natural kinds , 2010 .

[174]  S. Gelman,et al.  The Essential Child : Origins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought , 2003 .

[175]  P. Smolensky On the proper treatment of connectionism , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[176]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A single-system account of semantic and lexical deficits in five semantic dementia patients , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[177]  David M. Sobel,et al.  A theory of causal learning in children: causal maps and Bayes nets. , 2004, Psychological review.

[178]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  Emergentism—Use Often and With Care , 2006 .

[179]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object properties and knowledge in early lexical learning. , 1991, Child development.

[180]  H D Ellis,et al.  Accounting for Delusional Misidentifications , 1990, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[181]  E. Warrington Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology the Selective Impairment of Semantic Memory the Selective Impairment of Semantic Memory , 2022 .

[182]  G. Murphy,et al.  Changes in conceptual structure with expertise: Differences between real-world experts and novices. , 1984 .

[183]  Alex Lascarides,et al.  Temporal interpretation, discourse relations and commonsense entailment , 1993, The Language of Time - A Reader.

[184]  J. Mandler,et al.  Drinking and driving don't mix: inductive generalization in infancy , 1996, Cognition.

[185]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[186]  R. Nosofsky American Psychological Association, Inc. Choice, Similarity, and the Context Theory of Classification , 2022 .

[187]  James Woodward,et al.  Explanation and Invariance in the Special Sciences , 2000, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science.

[188]  Lance J. Rips,et al.  Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations , 1973 .

[189]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[190]  Vikash K. Mansinghka,et al.  Learning Cross-cutting Systems of Categories , 2006 .

[191]  D. Massaro Some criticisms of connectionist models of human performance , 1988 .

[192]  C. Mervis Child-basic object categories and early lexical development. , 1987 .

[193]  D Marr,et al.  Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. , 1971, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[194]  David I. Beaver,et al.  The Handbook of Logic and Language , 1997 .

[195]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Rethinking infant knowledge: toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. , 1997, Psychological review.

[196]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[197]  J J Hopfield,et al.  Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. , 1982, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[198]  Alice G. B. ter Meulen,et al.  Representing Time in Natural Language: The Dynamic Interpretation of Tense and Aspect , 1995 .

[199]  Anne L. Fulkerson,et al.  The Influence of Labels, Non-Labeling Sounds, and Source of Auditory Input on 9- and 15-Month-Olds' Object Categorization , 2003 .

[200]  P. Quinn Multiple sources of information and their integration, not dissociation, as an organizing framework for understanding infant concept formation , 2004 .

[201]  W. Ahn,et al.  Effect of theory-based feature correlations on typicality judgments , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[202]  J. Cohen,et al.  Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. , 1992, Psychological review.

[203]  C. Sumiyoshi CATEGORY BASED INDUCTION , 1997 .

[204]  J. Hampton A demonstration of intransitivity in natural categories , 1982, Cognition.

[205]  R. Dolan,et al.  A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[206]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning Representations by Recirculation , 1987, NIPS.

[207]  S. E. Robbins On time, memory and dynamic form , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[208]  G. Marcus Can connectionism save constructivism? , 1998, Cognition.

[209]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning and relearning in Boltzmann machines , 1986 .

[210]  P. Woit Not even wrong : the failure of string theory and the continuing challenge to unify the laws of physics , 2006 .

[211]  James L. McClelland Category Learning: Learning the general but not the specific , 1994, Current Biology.

[212]  Martial Mermillod,et al.  The Importance of Long-term Memory in Infant Perceptual Categorization , 2003 .

[213]  J. Kruschke,et al.  ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.

[214]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .

[215]  Lisa M. Oakes,et al.  Making Sense of Infant Categorization: Stable Processes and Changing Representations☆☆☆ , 1999 .

[216]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  A Learning Algorithm for Boltzmann Machines , 1985, Cogn. Sci..

[217]  Jonathan Evans The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[218]  Frank C Keil,et al.  What Do Children Want to Know About Animals and Artifacts? , 2006, Psychological science.

[219]  P. Brown ‘She had just cut/broken off her head’: Cutting and breaking verbs in Tzeltal , 2007 .

[220]  G. Lupyan,et al.  Developing object concepts in infancy: an associative learning perspective. , 2008, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[221]  David E. Rumelhart,et al.  Toward an interactive model of reading. , 1994 .

[222]  T. Shallice,et al.  Category specific semantic impairments. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[223]  G. Csibra,et al.  Alison Gopnik Andrew N. Meltroff , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[224]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Poverty of the Stimulus? A Rational Approach , 2006 .

[225]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Theory-based Bayesian models of inductive learning and reasoning , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[226]  H. Wellman,et al.  Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. , 1998 .

[227]  Bob Rehder,et al.  How causal knowledge affects classification: A generative theory of categorization. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[228]  J. Fodor The Mind Doesn't Work That Way : The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology , 2000 .

[229]  Arthur B. Markman,et al.  Analogy-- Watershed or Waterloo? Structural alignment and the development of connectionist models of analogy , 1992, NIPS 1992.

[230]  Patrick Shafto,et al.  Who is susceptible to conjunction fallacies in category-based induction? , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[231]  R. Nosofsky Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[232]  Christine D. Wilson,et al.  Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[233]  S. Shieber Subcognition and the Limits of the Turing Test , 2004 .

[234]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Learning Domain Structures , 2004 .

[235]  J. Elman,et al.  Learning and morphological change , 1995, Cognition.

[236]  J. Mandler,et al.  On developing a knowledge base in infancy. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[237]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: when does language become a tool for categorization? , 2001, Cognition.

[238]  L. Gogate,et al.  Attention and memory for faces and actions in infancy: the salience of actions over faces in dynamic events. , 2002, Child development.

[239]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Schemata and Sequential Thought Processes in PDP Models , 1986 .

[240]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Evidence for a global categorical representation of humans by young infants. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[241]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[242]  L. Rips,et al.  Necessity and natural categories. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[243]  Miriam Van Staden,et al.  How similar are semantic categories in closely related languages? A comparison of cutting and breaking in four Germanic languages , 2007 .