Measuring Implementation of a Structured Educational Model in an Urban School District: An Observational Approach

Most analyses of the National Follow Through Study (Stebbins et al., 1977; House et al., 1978) reported unexpectedly large degrees of variability in the results for each model. Several authors (House et al., 1978; Kennedy, 1978) have hypothesized that this variability was due in part to fluctuations in the extent to which a model was implemented at a given site. Yet only a small sample of classrooms was observed to measure fidelity of implementation in the original evaluation (Stallings, 1975) and those were not of students included in the Abt report. In many ways the Direct Instruction Follow Through Model is an ideal candidate for an implementation study. An almost fully articulated system exists (see, e.g., Becker, 1977; Becker et al., 1980), specifying in detail what teachers should be doing and saying, how much time should be spent on reading, arithmetic, and oral language each day; how fast lessons should be paced, and at what accuracy rate students should be responding. Thus, it was much more direct and simple to operationalize a behavioral coding system than to perform the same procedure on one of the open classroom models analyzed by the Stallings (1975) group in their Follow Through classroom research. Another unique component of the model is that the role of classroom aides is clearly specified-they are responsible for teaching academic curriculum material, usually oral language and arithmetic, to groups of 6 to 10 children. Thus, the same observation system can be used to analyze performance of both teachers and paraprofessionals.

[1]  Wesley C. Becker,et al.  Teaching Reading and Language to the Disadvantaged--What We Have Learned from Field Research. , 1977 .

[2]  B. Bloom Human Characteristics and School Learning , 1979 .

[3]  D. Carnine Effects of two teacher-presentation rates on off-task behavior, answering correctly, and participation. , 1976, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[4]  Douglas A. Grouws,et al.  The Missouri Mathematics Effectiveness Project: An experimental study in fourth-grade classrooms. , 1979 .

[5]  Gene E. Hall,et al.  A Developmental Model for Determining Whether the Treatment is Actually Implemented , 1977 .

[6]  E. Brunswik Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments , 1957 .

[7]  Richard J. Shavelson,et al.  Generalizability of Measures of Teaching Behavior , 1976 .

[8]  Thomas L. Good,et al.  Time on Task: A Naturalistic Study in Sixth-Grade Classrooms , 1978, The Elementary School Journal.

[9]  Gaea Leinhardt,et al.  The Instructional Dimensions Study , 1980 .

[10]  D. Carnine,et al.  Increasing the rate of presentation and use of signals in elementary classroom teachers. , 1978, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[11]  Decker Walker,et al.  No Simple Answer: Critique of the Follow Through Evaluation , 1978 .

[12]  D. Barlow,et al.  Single Case Experimental Designs: Strategies for Studying Behavior Change , 1976 .

[13]  Linda B. Stebbins,et al.  Education as Experimentation: A Planned Variation Model. Volume IIIA: Findings: Cohort II; Interim Findings: Cohort III. Volume IIIB: Appendices. , 1976 .

[14]  Richard E. Snow,et al.  Representative and quasi-representation designs for research on teaching. , 1974 .

[15]  Jane A. Stallings Follow Through program classroom observation evaluation 1971-72 , 1973 .

[16]  Benjamin B. Lahey,et al.  Advances in Clinical Child Psychology , 1980, Advances in Clinical Child Psychology.

[17]  Bernard Myers,et al.  The Scientific Basis of the Art of Teaching by N. L. Gage (review) , 1982 .

[18]  C. Evertson,et al.  Learning from Teaching: A Developmental Perspective , 1976 .

[19]  D. Berliner,et al.  Academic engaged time , 1978 .

[20]  Mary M. Kennedy,et al.  Findings from the Follow Through Planned Variation Study , 1978 .

[21]  A. Anastasi Psychological testing, 4th ed. , 1976 .