Time-dependent motor memory processes in amnesic subjects.

Functional properties of motor memory change with the passage of time. The time-dependent nature of memories in humans has also been demonstrated for certain "declarative" memories. When the declarative memory system is damaged, are the time-dependent properties associated with motor memories intact? To approach this question, we examined five subjects with global amnesia (AMN), including subject H.M., and a group of age-matched control subjects. The task was to make reaching movements to visually presented targets. We found that H.M. (but not the other subjects) was significantly impaired in the ability to perform the visuomotor kinematic transformations required in this task, to accurately move the hand in the direction specified by a target. With extensive practice, H.M.'s performance improved significantly. At this point, a force field was imposed on the hand. With practice in field A, H.M. and other AMN subjects developed aftereffects and maintained these aftereffects for 24 h. To quantify postpractice properties associated with motor memories, subjects learned field B on day 2 and at 5 min were retested in field A. In both subject groups, performance in field A was significantly worse than their own naive performance a day earlier. The aftereffects indicated persistence of the just-learned but now inappropriate motor memory. After 4 h of rest, subjects were retested in B. Performance was now at naive levels. The aftereffects at 4 h indicated a reduced influence of the memory of field A. The time-dependent patterns of motor memory perseveration, as measured at 5 min and 4 h, were not different in the AMN and normal control groups.

[1]  M. Van der Linden,et al.  Proactive interference in patients with amnesia resulting from anterior communicating artery aneurysm. , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[2]  G. Winocur,et al.  Heightened Interference on Implicit, but Not Explicit, Tests of Negative Transfer: Evidence from Patients with Unilateral Temporal Lobe Lesions and Normal Old People , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[3]  John D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer's disease and in global amnesia. , 1993 .

[4]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.

[5]  Elisabeth A. Murray,et al.  Role of the hippocampus plus subjacent cortex but not amygdala in visuomotor conditional learning in rhesus monkeys. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Gabriel Leonard,et al.  The effects of frontal- or temporal-lobe lesions on susceptibility to interference in spatial memory , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  S. Rapcsak,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Memory Following Anterior Communicating Artery Aneurysm Rupture , 1993, Brain and Cognition.

[9]  B. McGonigle,et al.  Serial Adaptation to Conflicting Prismatic Rearrangement Effects in Monkey and Man , 1977, Perception.

[10]  Perceptual-Motor Learning in Amnesic Patients with Medial Temporal Lobe Lesions , 1993, Perceptual and motor skills.

[11]  A. Karni,et al.  The time course of learning a visual skill , 1993, Nature.

[12]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interference in Learning Internal Models of Inverse Dynamics in Humans , 1994, NIPS.

[14]  L. Postman,et al.  Temporal changes in interference. , 1968 .

[15]  R. Worsham Temporal discrimination factors in the delayed matching-to-sample task in monkeys , 1975 .

[16]  S. Corkin Lasting Consequences of Bilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy: Clinical Course and Experimental Findings in H.M. , 1984 .

[17]  M. Hallett,et al.  Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system , 1983, Neurology.

[18]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Diminished ability to interpret and report internal states after bilateral medial temporal resection: case H.M. , 1985, Behavioral neuroscience.

[19]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[20]  S. Wise,et al.  The Acquisition of Motor Behavior in Vertebrates , 1996 .

[21]  J. M. Hofferer,et al.  Visual-motor adaptation. Quantitative demonstration in patients with posterior fossa involvement. , 1979, Archives of neurology.

[22]  B T Hyman,et al.  H. M.’s Medial Temporal Lobe Lesion: Findings from Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  A R Damasio,et al.  Sensorimotor skill learning in amnesia: additional evidence for the neural basis of nondeclarative memory. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[24]  W T Thach,et al.  The cerebellum and the adaptive coordination of movement. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  M. E. Bunch,et al.  Transfer of training in the mastery of an antagonistic habit after varying intervals of time. , 1939 .

[26]  D. Lewis,et al.  Facilitation and interference in performance on the modified Mashburn apparatus: I. The effects of varying the amount of original learning. , 1951, Journal of experimental psychology.

[27]  B. Underwood Proactive inhibition as a function of time and degree of prior learning. , 1949, Journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  Benton J. Underwood,et al.  Retroactive and proactive inhibition after five and forty-eight hours. , 1948 .

[29]  J. Brandt,et al.  An attempt at memory retraining in severe amnesia: An experimental single-case study , 1993 .

[30]  Suzanne Corkin,et al.  ACQUISITION OF MOTOR SKILL AFTER BILATERAL MEDIAL TEMPORAL-LOBE EXCISION , 1968 .

[31]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  A. Karni,et al.  Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. , 1994, Science.

[33]  P Alvarez,et al.  The animal model of human amnesia: long-term memory impaired and short-term memory intact. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  S. Butler,et al.  Does amnesia after transection of the fornix in monkeys reflect abnormal sensitivity to proactive interference? , 1984, Behavioural Brain Research.

[35]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[36]  D. Lewis,et al.  Evidences of Associative Interference in Psychomotor Performance. , 1949, Science.

[37]  R. Koppenaal,et al.  Time changes in the strengths of A–B, A–C lists; spontaneous recovery? , 1963 .

[38]  R. Passingham The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1993 .

[39]  Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Computational Elements of the Adaptive Controller of the Human Arm , 1993, NIPS.

[40]  Gordon Winocur,et al.  An investigation of paired-associate learning in amnesic patients , 1976, Neuropsychologia.