Perceiving depth order during pursuit eye movement

Pursuit eye movements alter retinal motion cues to depth. For instance, the sinusoidal retinal velocity profile produced by a translating, corrugated surface resembles a sinusoidal shear during pursuit. One way to recover the correct spatial phase of the corrugation's profile (i.e. which part is near and which part is far) is to combine estimates of shear with extra-retinal estimates of translation. In support of this hypothesis, we found the corrugation's spatial phase appeared ambiguous when retinal shear was viewed without translation, but unambiguous when translated and viewed with or without a pursuit eye movement. The eyes lagged the sinusoidal translation by a small but persistent amount, raising the possibility that retinal slip could serve as the disambiguating cue in the eye-moving condition. A yoked control was therefore performed in which measured horizontal slip was fed back into a fixated shearing stimulus on a trial-by-trial basis. The results showed that the corrugation's phase was only seen unambiguously during the real eye movement. This supports the idea that extra-retinal estimates of eye velocity can help disambiguate ordinal depth structure within moving retinal images.

[1]  G. B. Wetherill,et al.  SEQUENTIAL ESTIMATION OF POINTS ON A PSYCHOMETRIC FUNCTION. , 1965, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[2]  K. Hayashibe,et al.  Reversals of Visual Depth Caused by Motion Parallax , 1991, Perception.

[3]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Perception of self-motion from visual flow , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  C Caudek,et al.  Perceiving surface slant from deformation of optic flow. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Mark Nawrot,et al.  Eye movements provide the extra-retinal signal required for the perception of depth from motion parallax , 2003, Vision Research.

[6]  F. Domini,et al.  3-D structure perceived from dynamic information: a new theory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  J. Gibson,et al.  Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  Alexander H. Wertheim,et al.  Motion perception during selfmotion: The direct versus inferential controversy revisited , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  J C Liter,et al.  The relationship of vertical and horizontal velocity gradients in the perception of shape, rotation, and rigidity. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  M. Hogervorst,et al.  Spatial frequency tuning for 3-D corrugations from motion parallax , 2000, Vision Research.

[11]  D. Whitteridge Movements of the eyes R. H. S. Carpenter, Pion Ltd, London (1977), 420 pp., $27.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[12]  W. J. V. Damme,et al.  Non-visual Information in Structure-from-motion , 1996, Vision Research.

[13]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Self-motion and the perception of stationary objects , 2001, Nature.

[14]  H. Ono,et al.  Monocular stereopsis with and without head movement. , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  T. Freeman,et al.  Path perception and Filehne illusion compared: model and data , 1999, Vision Research.

[16]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[17]  James A. Crowell,et al.  The perception of heading during eye movements , 1992, Nature.

[18]  Y. Mori,et al.  New methods for removing saccades in analysis of smooth pursuit eye movement , 1988, Biological Cybernetics.

[19]  Martin S. Banks,et al.  Perceived head-centric speed is affected by both extra-retinal and retinal errors , 1998, Vision Research.

[20]  J. Droulez,et al.  The visual perception of three-dimensional shape from self-motion and object-motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[21]  J A Crowell,et al.  Extraretinal and retinal amplitude and phase errors during Filehne illusion and path perception , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  T. Meese,et al.  Computation of surface slant from optic flow: Orthogonal components of speed gradient can be combined , 1997, Vision Research.

[23]  Mike Harris,et al.  Retinal speed gradients and the perception of surface slant , 1992, Vision Research.

[24]  Mark F. Bradshaw,et al.  Sensitivity to horizontal and vertical corrugations defined by binocular disparity , 1999, Vision Research.

[25]  B Rogers,et al.  Motion Parallax as an Independent Cue for Depth Perception , 1979, Perception.

[26]  Hiroyasu Ujike,et al.  Depth thresholds of motion parallax as a function of head movement velocity , 2001, Vision Research.

[27]  G. Barnes,et al.  Visual-vestibular interaction in the control of head and eye movement: The role of visual feedback and predictive mechanisms , 1993, Progress in neurobiology.

[28]  J. Aloimonos,et al.  On the kinetic depth effect , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[29]  H. Ono,et al.  Apparent Depth with Motion Aftereffect and Head Movement , 1994, Perception.

[30]  Tim S. Meese,et al.  On the relationship between deformation and perceived surface slant , 1996, Vision Research.

[31]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[32]  T.C.A. Freeman,et al.  Unequal retinal and extra-retinal motion signals produce different perceived slants of moving surfaces , 2000, Vision Research.

[33]  Robert W. Massof,et al.  Nonlinear contribution of eye velocity to motion perception , 2001, Vision Research.

[34]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Human smooth and saccadic eye movements during voluntary pursuit of different target motions on different backgrounds. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[35]  M L Braunstein,et al.  The observer-relative velocity field as the basis for effective motion parallax. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  B J Rogers,et al.  The Appearance of Surfaces Specified by Motion Parallax and Binocular Disparity , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[37]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  Interactions between self-motion and depth perception in the processing of optic flow , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[38]  J. Droulez,et al.  The stationarity hypothesis: an allocentric criterion in visual perception , 2001, Vision Research.

[39]  B. Rogers,et al.  Similarities between motion parallax and stereopsis in human depth perception , 1982, Vision Research.

[40]  J. Gibson,et al.  Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. , 1955, The American journal of psychology.

[41]  R L Gregory The Aesthetics of Anaesthetics , 1979, Perception.

[42]  B J Rogers,et al.  Visual and nonvisual information disambiguate surfaces specified by motion parallax , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.