Modulation of cognitive control levels via manipulation of saccade trial-type probability assessed with event-related BOLD fMRI.

Cognitive control supports flexible behavior adapted to meet current goals and can be modeled through investigation of saccade tasks with varying cognitive demands. Basic prosaccades (rapid glances toward a newly appearing stimulus) are supported by neural circuitry, including occipital and posterior parietal cortex, frontal and supplementary eye fields, and basal ganglia. These trials can be contrasted with complex antisaccades (glances toward the mirror image location of a stimulus), which are characterized by greater functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal in the aforementioned regions and recruitment of additional regions such as dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The current study manipulated the cognitive demands of these saccade tasks by presenting three rapid event-related runs of mixed saccades with a varying probability of antisaccade vs. prosaccade trials (25, 50, or 75%). Behavioral results showed an effect of trial-type probability on reaction time, with slower responses in runs with a high antisaccade probability. Imaging results exhibited an effect of probability in bilateral pre- and postcentral gyrus, bilateral superior temporal gyrus, and medial frontal gyrus. Additionally, the interaction between saccade trial type and probability revealed a strong probability effect for prosaccade trials, showing a linear increase in activation parallel to antisaccade probability in bilateral temporal/occipital, posterior parietal, medial frontal, and lateral prefrontal cortex. In contrast, antisaccade trials showed elevated activation across all runs. Overall, this study demonstrated that improbable performance of a typically simple prosaccade task led to augmented BOLD signal to support changing cognitive control demands, resulting in activation levels similar to the more complex antisaccade task.

[1]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Canceling planned action: an FMRI study of countermanding saccades. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Dissociable effects of surprise and model update in parietal and anterior cingulate cortex , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  Dara S. Manoach,et al.  Switching, plasticity, and prediction in a saccadic task-switch paradigm , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  E. Stuyven,et al.  The effect of cognitive load on saccadic eye movements. , 2000, Acta psychologica.

[5]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[6]  S. Hutton Cognitive control of saccadic eye movements , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[7]  Benedikt Reuter,et al.  Distinct Neural Correlates for Volitional Generation and Inhibition of Saccades , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  Qingyang Li,et al.  Individual differences in working memory moderate the relationship between prosaccade latency and antisaccade error rate. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[9]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  EEG Source Reconstruction Reveals Frontal-Parietal Dynamics of Spatial Conflict Processing , 2013, PloS one.

[11]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Antisaccades as decisions: LATER model predicts latency distributions and error responses , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  S. Lomber,et al.  Effects of unilateral deactivations of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex on saccadic eye movements. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Matthew R. G. Brown,et al.  Neural processes associated with antisaccade task performance investigated with event-related FMRI. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  O. Tzeng,et al.  Trial type probability modulates the cost of antisaccades. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  B. J. McCurtain,et al.  Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: an fMRI study. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Carol L. Colby,et al.  Stimulus–Response Incompatibility Activates Cortex Proximate to Three Eye Fields , 2001, NeuroImage.

[18]  W. R. Hess,et al.  Motorische Funktion des Tektal- und Tegmentalgebietes; pp. 1–26 , 1946 .

[19]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[20]  S Monsell,et al.  Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? , 2000, Psychological research.

[21]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm , 2001, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[22]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[23]  J. McDowell,et al.  Trial-type probability and task-switching effects on behavioral response characteristics in a mixed saccade task , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Jazmin Camchong,et al.  An effect of context on saccade-related behavior and brain activity , 2007, NeuroImage.

[25]  D. Ffytche,et al.  Decomposing the neural correlates of antisaccade eye movements using event-related FMRI. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Matthew S. Cain,et al.  Neural Activity Is Modulated by Trial History: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of the Effects of a Previous Antisaccade , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Transient Attention Enhances Perceptual Performance and fMRI Response in Human Visual Cortex , 2005, Neuron.

[28]  M. Carrasco Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.

[29]  R. Roberts,et al.  Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. , 1994 .

[30]  Pierre-Alexandre Klein,et al.  Top-down suppression of incompatible motor activations during response selection under conflict , 2014, NeuroImage.

[31]  Matthew S. Cain,et al.  Rostral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex make dissociable contributions during antisaccade error commission , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  K. Johnston,et al.  Top-Down Control-Signal Dynamics in Anterior Cingulate and Prefrontal Cortex Neurons following Task Switching , 2007, Neuron.

[33]  D. Munoz,et al.  Reflex suppression in the anti-saccade task is dependent on prestimulus neural processes. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[35]  Stavros J. Perantonis,et al.  A neural model of decision-making by the superior colicullus in an antisaccade task , 2007, Neural Networks.

[36]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  D. Munoz,et al.  Saccadic Probability Influences Motor Preparation Signals and Time to Saccadic Initiation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in humans investigated with event-related FMRI. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Floris P. de Lange,et al.  Expectation Suppression in Early Visual Cortex Depends on Task Set , 2015, PloS one.

[41]  Cristina Massen,et al.  Parallel programming of exogenous and endogenous components in the antisaccade task , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[42]  Kristen A. Ford,et al.  Inhibition and generation of saccades: Rapid event-related fMRI of prosaccades, antisaccades, and nogo trials , 2006, NeuroImage.

[43]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[44]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Less Is More: Expectation Sharpens Representations in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[45]  B. Clementz,et al.  Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: Evidence from studies of humans , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[46]  Bernd Weber,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of sensorimotor transformations in saccades and antisaccades , 2014, NeuroImage.

[47]  Robert W. Cox,et al.  AFNI: What a long strange trip it's been , 2012, NeuroImage.

[48]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  Early visual cortex reflects initiation and maintenance of task set , 2015, NeuroImage.

[49]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[50]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[51]  John A. Sweeney,et al.  Reduced Attentional Engagement Contributes to Deficits in Prefrontal Inhibitory Control in Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[52]  M. Taira,et al.  Functional MRI mapping of brain activation during visually guided saccades and antisaccades: cortical and subcortical networks , 2004, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[53]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[54]  C. Curtis,et al.  Success and Failure Suppressing Reflexive Behavior , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Control of the superior colliculus by the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2013, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[56]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[57]  J. D. Connolly,et al.  Saccade preparation signals in the human frontal and parietal cortices. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Shintaro Funahashi,et al.  Saccade-related activity in the prefrontal cortex: its role in eye movement control and cognitive functions , 2014, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[59]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Frontoparietal activation with preparation for antisaccades. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[61]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[62]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Cognitive control processes during an anticipated switch of task , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[63]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Expected Value of Control: An Integrative Theory of Anterior Cingulate Cortex Function , 2013, Neuron.