Freely chosen and instructed actions are terminated by different neural mechanisms revealed by kinematics-informed EEG

ABSTRACT Neurophysiological accounts of human volition are dominated by debates on the origin of voluntary choices but the neural consequences that follow such choices remain poorly understood. For instance, could one predict whether or not an action was chosen voluntarily based only on how that action is motorically executed? We investigated this possibility by integrating scalp electroencephalograms and index‐finger accelerometer recordings acquired while people chose between pressing a left or right button either freely or as instructed by a visual cue. Even though freely selected and instructed actions were executed with equal vigor, the timing of the movement to release the button was comparatively delayed for freely selected actions. This chronometric difference was six‐times larger for the &bgr;‐oscillations over the sensorimotor cortex that characteristically accompany an action's termination. This surprising modulation of an action's termination by volition was traceable to volition‐modulated differences in how the competing yet non‐selected action was represented and regulated. HIGHLIGHTSA voluntary action choice modulated the action's neural and behavioral execution.Volition's motoric effects were prominent during action termination.Index finger actions that were freely chosen ended 40ms later than when instructed.&bgr;‐rebound onset for free actions occurred 240ms later than for instructed actions.Inter‐hemispheric motor coupling had opposite phases for free and instructed actions.

[1]  Margaret T. Lynn,et al.  Imaging volition: what the brain can tell us about the will , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Sukhvinder S. Obhi,et al.  Internally generated and externally triggered actions are physically distinct and independently controlled , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  What are self-generated actions? , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[4]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Methods for Dealing With Reaction Time Outliers , 1992 .

[5]  Stefan Bode,et al.  Similar neural mechanisms for perceptual guesses and free decisions , 2013, NeuroImage.

[6]  P. Haggard,et al.  On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  G. Aschersleben Temporal Control of Movements in Sensorimotor Synchronization , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[8]  D. Cheyne MEG studies of sensorimotor rhythms: A review , 2013, Experimental Neurology.

[9]  W. Prinz,et al.  Neural and behavioral correlates of intentional actions , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  P. Nunez,et al.  A theoretical and experimental study of high resolution EEG based on surface Laplacians and cortical imaging. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[11]  Julie Duque,et al.  Physiological Markers of Motor Inhibition during Human Behavior , 2017, Trends in Neurosciences.

[12]  R. Passingham,et al.  Attention to Intention , 2004, Science.

[13]  Sukhvinder S. Obhi,et al.  Modi W cation of planned actions , 2008 .

[14]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  F. Perrin,et al.  Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  M. Brass,et al.  Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  John-Dylan Haynes,et al.  Similar coding of freely chosen and externally cued intentions in a fronto-parietal network , 2016, NeuroImage.

[18]  Aaron Schurger,et al.  Nowhere and Everywhere: The Causal Origin of Voluntary Action , 2015 .

[19]  P. Cisek,et al.  Deliberation and Commitment in the Premotor and Primary Motor Cortex during Dynamic Decision Making , 2014, Neuron.

[20]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Use of tactile afferent information in sequential finger movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  B. Libet,et al.  Readiness-potentials preceding unrestricted 'spontaneous' vs. pre-planned voluntary acts. , 1982, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .

[23]  G. Fink,et al.  Differential effects of dopaminergic medication on basic motor performance and executive functions in Parkinson's disease , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  K. Zilles,et al.  The "what" and "when" of self-initiated movements. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  Parashkev Nachev,et al.  Volition and Conflict in Human Medial Frontal Cortex , 2005, Current Biology.

[26]  M. Brass,et al.  The What, When, Whether Model of Intentional Action , 2008, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[27]  Silvia Daun-Gruhn,et al.  Movement-related phase locking in the delta–theta frequency band , 2016, NeuroImage.

[28]  Walter J. Freeman,et al.  Origin, structure, and role of background EEG activity. Part 1. Analytic amplitude , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[29]  A. Roskies,et al.  How does neuroscience affect our conception of volition? , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  E. Donchin,et al.  Preparation to respond as manifested by movement-related brain potentials , 1980, Brain Research.

[31]  A. Labarga,et al.  Alpha and beta oscillatory activity during a sequence of two movements , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[32]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  The role of the preSMA and the rostral cingulate zone in internally selected actions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[33]  M. Desmurget,et al.  A parietal-premotor network for movement intention and motor awareness , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  A. Mognon,et al.  ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[35]  Angelika Lingnau,et al.  Decoding Internally and Externally Driven Movement Plans , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  KongFatt Wong-Lin,et al.  Bridging Neural and Computational Viewpoints on Perceptual Decision-Making , 2018, Trends in Neurosciences.

[37]  S. Grondin Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[38]  Antti Oulasvirta,et al.  Neuromechanics of a Button Press , 2018, CHI.

[39]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[40]  L. Deecke,et al.  The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[41]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Borís Burle,et al.  Preventing (impulsive) errors: Electrophysiological evidence for online inhibitory control over incorrect responses , 2016, Psychophysiology.

[43]  R. Passingham,et al.  Medial frontal cortex: from self-generated action to reflection on one's own performance , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  C. Tenke,et al.  Principal components analysis of Laplacian waveforms as a generic method for identifying ERP generator patterns: II. Adequacy of low-density estimates , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[45]  François Thénault,et al.  Delta-Band Oscillations in Motor Regions Predict Hand Selection for Reaching , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  G. Fink,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of motor network dynamics in Parkinson’s disease , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  Stefan Bode,et al.  Demystifying “free will”: The role of contextual information and evidence accumulation for predictive brain activity , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[49]  M. Desmurget,et al.  Movement Intention After Parietal Cortex Stimulation in Humans , 2009, Science.

[50]  S. Schütz-Bosbach,et al.  One Action System or Two? Evidence for Common Central Preparatory Mechanisms in Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Actions , 2011, Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Sam J. Gilbert,et al.  Modification of planned actions , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  P. Fox,et al.  The role of anterior midcingulate cortex in cognitive motor control , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[53]  Sukhvinder Obhi,et al.  Modification of planned actions , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Physiological evidence for response inhibition in choice reaction time tasks , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[55]  A. Allport,et al.  Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .

[56]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  Tapping with peripheral nerve block , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  Shivakumar Viswanathan,et al.  Frequency-specific modulation of connectivity in the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex by different forms of movement initiation , 2017, NeuroImage.

[58]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[59]  U. Castiello,et al.  The kinematic signature of voluntary actions , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  Philipp Berens,et al.  CircStat: AMATLABToolbox for Circular Statistics , 2009, Journal of Statistical Software.

[61]  A. Riehle,et al.  The ups and downs of beta oscillations in sensorimotor cortex , 2013, Experimental Neurology.

[62]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[63]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional cerebral blood flow. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[64]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983 .

[65]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[66]  Flavio T. P. Oliveira,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation of posterior parietal cortex affects decisions of hand choice , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[67]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Dynamic intra- and interhemispheric interactions during unilateral and bilateral hand movements assessed with fMRI and DCM , 2008, NeuroImage.

[68]  Caroline Palmer,et al.  Tactile feedback and timing accuracy in piano performance , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[69]  J. F. Schouten,et al.  Reaction time and accuracy. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[70]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[71]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Willed action and the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[72]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[73]  Andrew M. Gordon,et al.  Tactile feedback contributes to consistency of finger movements during typing , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[74]  N. Erbil,et al.  Changes in the alpha and beta amplitudes of the central EEG during the onset, continuation, and offset of long-duration repetitive hand movements , 2007, Brain Research.

[75]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  The illusion of conscious will , 2007, Synthese.

[76]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[77]  P. Derambure,et al.  Brief and sustained movements: differences in event-related (de)synchronization (ERD/ERS) patterns , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[78]  Thilo Van Eimeren,et al.  Implementation of visuospatial cues in response selection , 2006, NeuroImage.

[79]  M. Hallett,et al.  What is the Bereitschaftspotential? , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[80]  E. Paulesu,et al.  The What, the When, and the Whether of Intentional Action in the Brain: A Meta-Analytical Review , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[81]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[82]  Walter J. Freeman,et al.  Origin, structure, and role of background EEG activity. Part 3. Neural frame classification , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[83]  L. Selen,et al.  Deliberation in the Motor System: Reflex Gains Track Evolving Evidence Leading to a Decision , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[84]  Michael T. Jurkiewicz,et al.  Post-movement beta rebound is generated in motor cortex: Evidence from neuromagnetic recordings , 2006, NeuroImage.

[85]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  Intention-based and stimulus-based mechanisms in action selection , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[86]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[87]  Parashkev Nachev,et al.  Action and the fallacy of the ‘internal’: Comment on Passingham et al , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[88]  P. Haggard Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.