Recruitment of degrees of freedom stabilizes coordination.

By showing that transitions may be obviated by recruiting degrees of freedom in the coupled pendulum paradigm, the authors reveal a novel mechanism for coordinative flexibility. In Experiment 1, participants swung pairs of unconstrained pendulums in 2 planes of motion (sagittal and frontal) at 8 movement frequencies starting from either an in-phase or antiphase mode. Few transitions were observed. Measures of spatial trajectory showed recruitment effects tied to the stability of the initial coordinative pattern. When the motion of the pendulums was physically restricted to a single plane in Experiment 2, transitions were more common, indicating that recruitment delays--or even eliminates--transitions. Such recruitment complements transitions as a source of coordinative flexibility and is incorporated in a simple extension of the Haken-Kelso-Bunz (1985) model.

[1]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping , 1996 .

[2]  J. A. S. Kelso,et al.  Cooperative Phenomena in Biological Motion , 1985 .

[3]  F. Huddle Coordination , 1966, Open Knowledge Institutions.

[4]  J. Kelso,et al.  Learning as change of coordination dynamics: theory and experiment. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  J. Kelso,et al.  Action-Perception as a Pattern Formation Process , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[7]  M. Turvey From Boreli (1680) and Bell (1826) to the Dynamics of Action and Perception. , 1994, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[8]  J A Kelso,et al.  Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. , 1988, Science.

[9]  E. Saltzman,et al.  Steady-state and perturbed rhythmical movements: a dynamical analysis. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Lowell J. Anderson,et al.  To Switch or Not to Switch , 1994 .

[11]  Mingzhou Ding,et al.  Spontaneous recruitment and annihilation of degrees of freedom in biological coordination , 1993 .

[12]  J. Kelso,et al.  Multifunctionality and switching in the coordination dynamics of reaching and grasping , 1994 .

[13]  G. Ermentrout Principles of brain functioning: A synergetic approach to brain activity, behavior, and cognition , 1997 .

[14]  J. Kelso,et al.  Phase Transitions and Critical Fluctuations in Rhythmic Coordination of Ipsilateral Hand and Foot. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  J. Kelso,et al.  The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics , 1994 .

[16]  J. Kelso,et al.  Dynamics governs switching among patterns of coordination in biological movement , 1988 .

[17]  M. Turvey,et al.  Coupling dynamics in interlimb coordination. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[19]  G Schöner,et al.  A synergetic theory of quadrupedal gaits and gait transitions. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.

[20]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[21]  G. Ermentrout Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior , 1997 .

[22]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination , 1994 .

[23]  P. N. Kugler,et al.  Fluctuations and phase symmetry in coordinated rhythmic movements. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  P. V. Wieringen,et al.  Limit cycle properties of rhythmic forearm movements. , 1996 .

[25]  E. Saltzman,et al.  Space-time behavior of single and bimanual rhythmical movements: data and limit cycle model. , 1987 .

[26]  M. Turvey,et al.  Models of interlimb coordination--equilibria, local analyses, and spectral patterning: comment on Fuchs and Kelso (1994). , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  M. Turvey,et al.  Dynamics of bimanual rhythmic coordination in the coronal plane , 1997 .

[28]  M. T. Turvey,et al.  A Low-Dimensional Nonlinear Dynamic Governing Interlimb Rhythmic Coordination , 1994 .

[29]  J. A. S. Kelso,et al.  Synergetic Dynamics of Biological Coordination with Special Reference to Phase Attraction and Intermittency , 1991 .

[30]  J. Kelso,et al.  Symmetry breaking dynamics of human multilimb coordination. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  R. H. Wimmers,et al.  Phase transitions in rhythmic tracking movements: A case of unilateral coupling , 1992 .

[32]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  The Dynamical Substructure of Bimanual Coordination , 1994 .

[33]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Self-organization of trajectory formation , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[34]  M. Turvey,et al.  Diffusive, Synaptic, and Synergetic Coupling: An Evaluation Through In-Phase and Antiphase Rhythmic Movements. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[35]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Attention and Handedness in Bimanual Coordination Dynamics , 1997 .

[36]  J. Kelso,et al.  Toward a Physical (Synergetic) Theory of Biological Coordination , 1987 .

[37]  J. J. Buchanan,et al.  Self-organization of trajectory formation , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[38]  Mingzhou Ding,et al.  The spontaneous recruitment and suppression of degrees of freedom in rhythmic hand movements , 1997 .

[39]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Dynamics of human intersegmental coordination: Theory and research , 1998 .