Differentiating environmental concern in the context of psychological adaption to climate change

Abstract Despite existing evidence for the threats of climate change facing people living in the U.S., the psychological impacts of this threat have been neglected in public and scientific discourse, resulting in a notable lack in studies on individuals’ adaptation to climate change. Using social-cognitive theory, we examine how three forms of environmental concern—egoistic (e.g., concern for oneself; one’s health or life), social-altruistic (e.g., concern for others; future generations or country), and biospheric (e.g., concern for plants and animals; nature)—influence concurrent ecological stress and ecological coping strategies. Further, we examine how ecological stress and coping are associated with both depressive symptoms and pro-environmental behaviors. In an online survey of 342 U.S. adults we found unique patterns of the three forms of environmental concern. Only individuals higher in biospheric environmental concern perceived ecological stress and engaged in ecological coping. In contrast, individuals higher in social-altruistic concern did not perceive ecological stress, but did engage in ecological coping. Those higher in egoistic concern neither perceived ecological stress, nor engaged in coping. In addition, perceived ecological stress was positively associated with depressive symptoms; ecological coping negatively predicted depressive symptoms, while positively predicting pro-environmental behaviors. In sum, with the exception of those high in biospheric concern, study participants did not seem to perceive climate change threats as having a profound effect on their own or their family’s life. Differentiating three forms of environmental concern provides a nuanced view on their association with ecological stress and coping, and in turn depressive symptoms and pro-environmental behaviors. Results indicate that current public policy approaches that often focus on the natural environment when depicting or explaining the effects of climate change, may limit the effectiveness of interventions to those people who already show high concern for all living creatures, while failing to affect those motivated by egoistic or altruistic concern, increasing the risks associated with delaying climate change adaptation and the potential for large-scale negative mental health effects in our society.

[1]  R. Gifford,et al.  The largely unacknowledged impact of climate change on mental health , 2016 .

[2]  Thomas Dietz,et al.  Measuring household energy efficiency behaviors with attention to behavioral plasticity in the United States , 2015 .

[3]  Glenn Albrecht,et al.  Chronic environmental change: Emerging ‘psychoterratic’ syndromes , 2011 .

[4]  Daniel C. Molden,et al.  Motivations for promotion and prevention , 2008 .

[5]  J. Reser,et al.  Natural disasters, climate change and mental health considerations for rural Australia. , 2007, The Australian journal of rural health.

[6]  S. Capstick,et al.  Impacts of adaptation and responsibility framings on attitudes towards climate change mitigation , 2016, Climatic Change.

[7]  J. Reser,et al.  The Threat of Climate Change: Psychological Response, Adaptation, and Impacts , 2011 .

[8]  G. Bradley,et al.  Coping with climate change: bringing psychological adaptation in from the cold , 2012 .

[9]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. , 2010, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[10]  F. Fahy,et al.  Different shades of green? Unpacking habitual and occasional pro-environmental behavior , 2015 .

[11]  S. Schwartz Normative Influences on Altruism , 1977 .

[12]  Erik Blair,et al.  Social mobilization of climate change: University students conserving energy through multiple pathways for peer engagement , 2014 .

[13]  Tim Kasser,et al.  Meeting Environmental Challenges: The Role of Human Identity , 2009 .

[14]  A. Balmford,et al.  Conserving nature out of fear or knowledge? Using threatening versus connecting messages to generate support for environmental causes , 2015 .

[15]  Thomas Dietz,et al.  Values, Beliefs, and Proenvironmental Action: Attitude Formation Toward Emergent Attitude Objects1 , 1995 .

[16]  T. Little,et al.  To Parcel or Not to Parcel: Exploring the Question, Weighing the Merits , 2002 .

[17]  A. Ian Glendon,et al.  Public risk perceptions, understandings and responses to climate change in Australia and Great Britain , 2012 .

[18]  A. Homburg,et al.  Explaining pro-environmental behavior with a cognitive theory of stress , 2006 .

[19]  K. Norgaard Living in Denial: Climate Change, Emotions, and Everyday Life , 2011, International Dialogue.

[20]  L. Steg Values, Norms, and Intrinsic Motivation to Act Proenvironmentally , 2016 .

[21]  Dennis Fitzgerald,et al.  Do people “personally experience” global warming, and if so how, and does it matter? , 2013 .

[22]  D. Uzzell,et al.  Introduction: Environmental psychology on the move , 2009 .

[23]  Todd P. Newman,et al.  A re-assessment of factors associated with environmental concern and behavior using the 2010 General Social Survey , 2016 .

[24]  R. MacCallum,et al.  Model modifications in covariance structure analysis: the problem of capitalization on chance. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  G. Evans,et al.  Coping with multiple stressors in the environment. , 1996 .

[26]  Michael D. Buhrmester,et al.  Amazon's Mechanical Turk , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[27]  Psychological Adaptation to the Threats and Stresses of a Four Degree World , 2009 .

[28]  Ray W. Cooksey,et al.  Preaching to different choirs: How to motivate dismissive, uncommitted, and alarmed audiences to adapt to climate change? , 2016 .

[29]  C. Kormos,et al.  The validity of self-report measures of proenvironmental behavior: A meta-analytic review , 2014 .

[30]  John Thøgersen,et al.  Spillover processes in the development of a sustainable consumption pattern , 1999 .

[31]  Timothy O'Riordan,et al.  The psychology of denial concerning climate mitigation measures: evidence from Swiss focus groups , 2001 .

[32]  W. Poortinga,et al.  Values, Environmental Concern, and Environmental Behavior , 2004 .

[33]  N. Branscombe,et al.  Collective guilt mediates the effect of beliefs about global warming on willingness to engage in mitigation behavior , 2010 .

[34]  Sophie A. Nicholson-Cole Promoting Positive Engagement With Climate Change Through Visual and Iconic Representations , 2009 .

[35]  P. Stern,et al.  The Value Basis of Environmental Concern , 1994 .

[36]  L. Steg,et al.  An Integrated Framework for Encouraging Pro-environmental Behaviour: The role of values, situational factors and goals , 2014 .

[37]  P. Schultz New Environmental Theories: Empathizing With Nature: The Effects ofPerspective Taking on Concern for Environmental Issues , 2000 .

[38]  C. Leach,et al.  Experimental evidence for a dual pathway model analysis of coping with the climate crisis , 2010 .

[39]  P. Stern,et al.  Value Orientations, Gender, and Environmental Concern , 1993 .

[40]  S. Clayton,et al.  The psychological impacts of global climate change. , 2011, The American psychologist.

[41]  P. Schultz THE STRUCTURE OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONCERN: CONCERN FOR SELF, OTHER PEOPLE, AND THE BIOSPHERE , 2001 .

[42]  J. Swim,et al.  Adapting to and coping with the threat and impacts of climate change. , 2011, The American psychologist.

[43]  S. Bamberg,et al.  Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour , 2007 .

[44]  Panagiotis G. Ipeirotis,et al.  Running Experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk , 2010, Judgment and Decision Making.

[45]  C. Vlek,et al.  Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour : An integrative review and research agenda , 2009 .

[46]  A. Patt,et al.  Adaptive capacity and human cognition: The process of individual adaptation to climate change , 2005 .

[47]  Amar Cheema,et al.  Data collection in a flat world: the strengths and weaknesses of mechanical turk samples , 2013 .

[48]  Tim Jackson Live Better by Consuming Less?: Is There a “Double Dividend” in Sustainable Consumption? , 2005 .

[49]  U. Wagner,et al.  Coping With Global Environmental Problems , 2007 .

[50]  P. Maiteny,et al.  Mind in the Gap: Summary of research exploring 'inner' influences on pro-sustainability learning and behaviour , 2002 .

[51]  Nicholas Frank Pidgeon,et al.  Framing and Communicating Climate Change: The Effects of Distance and Outcome Frame Manipulations , 2010 .

[52]  L. Radloff The CES-D Scale , 1977 .

[53]  Daniel Stokols,et al.  Psychology in an age of ecological crisis: from personal angst to collective action. , 2009, American Psychologist.

[54]  Craig K. Enders,et al.  Applied Missing Data Analysis , 2010 .

[55]  R. Gifford,et al.  Psychology and Global Climate Change: Addressing a Multi-faceted Phenomenon and Set of Challenges A Report by the American Psychological Association's Task Force on the Interface Between Psychology and Global Climate Change Members , 2009 .

[56]  I. Knez Is Climate Change a Moral Issue? Effects of Egoism and Altruism on Pro-Environmental Behavior , 2016 .

[57]  S. Hales,et al.  Climate change and human health: present and future risks , 2006, The Lancet.

[58]  Lois A. Mohr,et al.  A re-examination of socially responsible consumption and its measurement☆ , 2008 .

[59]  E. Matthies,et al.  Responsibility and Environment , 2001 .

[60]  Cameron Brick,et al.  Unearthing the “Green” Personality , 2016 .

[61]  Susie Burke,et al.  Hope, despair and transformation: Climate change and the promotion of mental health and wellbeing , 2008, International journal of mental health systems.

[62]  W. Adger,et al.  Successful adaptation to climate change across scales , 2005 .