The posterior superior temporal sulcus is involved in social communication not specific for the eyes

Neuroimaging and lesion studies suggest that the superior temporal sulcus (STS) region is involved in eye gaze processing. Hence, the STS region is suggested to be the location of the "eye-direction detector", a key element in the "mindreading model" proposed by Baron-Cohen [Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: The MIT Press]. Not only the eyes, but also a pointing finger of another person can inform us about the direction of attention of the other one. In an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment, healthy human subjects actively followed a directional cue provided either by the eyes or, alternatively, the pointing finger of another person to make an eye movement toward an object in space. Our results show clearly that the posterior STS region is equally involved in processing directional information from either source. The only difference between the two cues was found in the lingual gyrus, in which a stronger blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response was observed during the finger pointing compared to the gaze following task. We suggest that different structures might be involved in the initial processing of directional information coming from the eyes or the pointing finger. These different streams of information may then converge in the posterior STS region, orchestrating the usage of a wider range of socially relevant directional cues able to inform us about the direction of attention and the intentions of another person.

[1]  T. Allison,et al.  Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  V. Bruce,et al.  You must see the point: automatic processing of cues to the direction of social attention. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  F. Curcio,et al.  Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children , 1978, Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia.

[4]  T. Allison,et al.  Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  How Many Subjects Constitute a Study? , 1999, NeuroImage.

[6]  J. Decety,et al.  Brain Regions Involved in the Perception of Gaze: A PET Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[7]  Richard S. Frackowiak,et al.  Superior temporal sulcus anatomical abnormalities in childhood autism: a voxel-based morphometry MRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[8]  D. Perrett,et al.  Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[10]  Arthur W Toga,et al.  Cortical sulcal maps in autism. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Tony Charman,et al.  Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[13]  Rieko Osu,et al.  The Neural Substrates of Biological Motion Perception: an fMRI Study , 2022 .

[14]  Kate Nation,et al.  Sensitivity to eye gaze in autism: Is it normal? Is it automatic? Is it social? , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.

[15]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[16]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Gaze Perception Triggers Reflexive Visuospatial Orienting , 1999 .

[17]  Peter Thier,et al.  Dissociable roles of the superior temporal sulcus and the intraparietal sulcus in joint attention: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2008 .

[18]  J. Haxby,et al.  Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Covert and overt orienting to gaze direction cues and the effects of fixation offset , 2003, Neuroreport.

[20]  K. Paller,et al.  Brain networks for analyzing eye gaze. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[21]  S. Langton,et al.  The Mutual Influence of Gaze and Head Orientation in the Analysis of Social Attention Direction , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[22]  H. Roeyers,et al.  Temporal Coordination of Joint Attention Behavior in Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[23]  V. Bruce,et al.  Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  A. Cowey,et al.  Sensitivity to eye gaze in prosopagnosic patients and monkeys with superior temporal sulcus ablation , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[27]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[28]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism , 1989 .

[29]  N. Emery,et al.  The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[30]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction , 1985, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[31]  G. McCarthy,et al.  When Strangers Pass , 2004, Psychological science.

[32]  D I Perrett,et al.  Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[33]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[34]  Tony Charman,et al.  Psychological Markers in the Detection of Autism in Infancy in a Large Population , 1996, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[35]  I. Wilkinson,et al.  Introduction to functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1999 .