So Far So Good: Emotion in the Peripersonal/Extrapersonal Space
暂无分享,去创建一个
Francisco J. Román | J. Hinojosa | F. J. Román | Berenice Valdés-Conroy | Berenice Valdés-Conroy | Jose A. Hinojosa | S. Paul Shorkey | S. P. Shorkey
[1] Sian L. Beilock,et al. Reach For What You Like: The Body's Role in Shaping Preferences , 2009 .
[2] S. Tipper,et al. Self produced and observed actions influence emotion: the roles of action fluency and eye gaze , 2008, Psychological research.
[3] J. Witt. Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serve as indirect measures of perceived distance. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al. The effects of handedness and reachability on perceived distance. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] R. Ellis,et al. Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.
[6] D. Perani,et al. Unilateral neglect: Personal and extra-personal , 1986, Neuropsychologia.
[7] Nicolas A. McNair,et al. Disentangling the contributions of grasp and action representations in the recognition of manipulable objects , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[8] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. I. Somatosensory responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.
[9] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[10] F. Pavani,et al. Action-specific remapping of peripersonal space , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[11] Valeria I. Petkova,et al. fMRI Adaptation Reveals a Cortical Mechanism for the Coding of Space Near the Hand , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] C. Spence,et al. Crossmodal Space and Crossmodal Attention , 2004 .
[13] M. Costantini,et al. When objects are close to me: Affordances in the peripersonal space , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[14] W. Epstein,et al. Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] M. Costantini,et al. Author ' s personal copy Short Communication Which body for embodied cognition ? Affordance and language within actual and perceived reaching space , 2012 .
[16] M. H. Fischer,et al. Estimating reachability: Whole body engagement or postural stability? , 2000 .
[17] J. Marshall,et al. Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.
[18] M. Bassolino,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Extended Multisensory Space in Blind Cane Users , 2022 .
[19] Michael D. Robinson,et al. Approach motivation as incentive salience: perceptual sources of evidence in relation to positive word primes. , 2012, Emotion.
[20] Emily Balcetis,et al. Fondness makes the distance grow shorter: Desired locations seem closer because they seem more vivid , 2011 .
[21] Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al. Asymmetrical Body Perception , 2009, Psychological science.
[22] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.
[23] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.
[24] Nicholas P. Holmes,et al. The body schema and multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space , 2004, Cognitive Processing.
[25] R. Passingham,et al. That's My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb , 2004, Science.
[26] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.
[27] F. Osiurak,et al. Tool use and perceived distance: when unreachable becomes spontaneously reachable , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] K. Zilles,et al. Polymodal Motion Processing in Posterior Parietal and Premotor Cortex A Human fMRI Study Strongly Implies Equivalencies between Humans and Monkeys , 2001, Neuron.
[29] M. Turvey,et al. Visually perceiving what is reachable. , 1989 .
[30] Andrea Serino,et al. Everyday use of the computer mouse extends peripersonal space representation , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[31] A. Bayliss,et al. Grasping the concept of personal property , 2011, Cognition.
[32] Valeria I. Petkova,et al. Integration of visual and tactile signals from the hand in the human brain: an FMRI study. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[33] R. Ellis,et al. On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] A. Berti,et al. When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[35] M S Graziano,et al. A System of Multimodal Areas in the Primate Brain , 2001, Neuron.
[36] Elisabetta Ladavas amp Farne,et al. Neuropsychological evidence for multimodal representations of space near specific body parts , 2004 .
[37] E. Macaluso,et al. The representation of space near the body through touch and vision , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[38] Emily Balcetis,et al. Wishful Seeing , 2010, Psychological science.
[39] Alice C. Roy,et al. Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema , 2009, Current Biology.
[40] R. Ellis,et al. Action priming by briefly presented objects. , 2004, Acta psychologica.
[41] Carl Gabbard,et al. Examining the Effects of Postural Constraints on Estimating Reach , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.
[42] Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al. The Role of Effort in Perceiving Distance , 2003, Psychological science.
[43] A. Markman,et al. Constraining Theories of Embodied Cognition , 2005, Psychological science.
[44] Ehud Zohary,et al. Is That Near My Hand? Multisensory Representation of Peripersonal Space in Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[45] K. Coventry,et al. Language within your reach: Near–far perceptual space and spatial demonstratives , 2008, Cognition.