Parallels between remembering and predicting an object's location

Three experiments explore effects of rotational axis and velocity on observers' predictions regarding an object's future position, and begin to explore connections between this extrapolation task and representational momentum (RM). In general, observers accept as "correct" positions that are behind the correct extrapolated location, as shown in previous work (Cooper & Munger, 1993; Finke & Shyi, 1998). These prediction distortions are in the opposite direction of typical RM memory distortions, suggesting that these tasks are unrelated. However, new effects of axis for the extrapolation task are reported which parallel axis effects previously observed for RM and mental rotation tasks. Specifically, participants make larger negative distortion errors for axes of rotation that are not coincident with the viewer's coordinate system, as if these paths of rotation are harder to extrapolate. Effects of velocity and axis are examined for RM and extrapolation tasks, and the overall pattern of distortions supports a link. In particular, participants who misremember the location of an object as further along the implied path of motion also accept as "correct" positions relatively further along the extrapolated path of motion, suggesting that these apparently opposite types of errors are indeed related.

[1]  T. L. Hubbard,et al.  Displacement in depth: Representational momentum and boundary extension , 1996, Psychological research.

[2]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  A matter of design: No representational momentum without predictability , 2002 .

[3]  Jennifer J. Freyd,et al.  Representational momentum when attention is divided , 2002 .

[4]  J. Freyd,et al.  Representational momentum for a spiral path. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  L. Parsons Inability to Reason About an Object's Orientation Using an Axis and Angle of Rotation , 1995 .

[6]  Jennifer J. Freyd,et al.  Dynamic Representations Guiding Adaptive Behavior , 1992 .

[7]  J. Freyd,et al.  A velocity effect for representational momentum , 1985 .

[8]  T. Hubbard,et al.  Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  M P Munger,et al.  Representational momentum for rotations in depth: effects of shadings and axis. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  David E. Meyer,et al.  Five Hunches about Perceptual Processes and Dynamic Representations , 1993 .

[11]  Lynn A. Cooper,et al.  Extrapolating and remembering positions along cognitive trajectories: Uses and limitations of analogies to physical motion , 1999 .

[12]  The relationship between mental rotation and representational momentum. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  Timothy L. Hubbard,et al.  Target size and displacement along the axis of implied gravitational attraction : Effects of implied weight and evidence of representational gravity , 1997 .

[14]  J. Freyd,et al.  Probing the time course of representational momentum. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[15]  M. Jones,et al.  Attending to auditory events: The role of temporal organization. , 1993 .

[16]  R. Finke,et al.  Mental extrapolation and representational momentum for complex implied motions. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.