Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A-not-B error beyond infancy: three-year-olds' delay- and experience-dependent location memory biases.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] H.L.J. van der Maas,et al. Detecting and modelling developmental transitions , 1998 .
[2] Philip David Zelazo,et al. The A‐Not‐B Error: Results from a Logistic Meta‐Analysis , 1999 .
[3] Y. Munakata. Article with Peer Commentaries and Response Infant Perseveration and Implications for Object Permanence Theories: a Pdp Model of the Ab B Task , 2022 .
[4] J. Piaget. The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .
[5] P Wenderoth,et al. Two-Dimensional Tilt Illusions Induced by Orthogonal Plaid Patterns: Effects of Plaid Motion, Orientation, Spatial Separation, and Spatial Frequency , 1989, Perception.
[6] H.L.J. van der Maas,et al. Stagewise cognitive development: an application of catastrophe theory. , 1992, Psychological review.
[7] S. Amari,et al. Competition and Cooperation in Neural Nets , 1982 .
[8] John P. Spencer,et al. Developmental Changes in the Relative Weighting of Geometric and Experience-Dependent Location Cues , 2003 .
[9] J. Huttenlocher,et al. The Coding of Spatial Location in Young Children , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.
[10] John P Spencer,et al. Prototypes and particulars: geometric and experience-dependent spatial categories. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[11] P. Wenderoth. THE ROLE OF IMPLICIT AXES OF BILATERAL SYMMETRY IN ORIENTATION PROCESSING , 1997 .
[12] A. Hartley,et al. The roles of axes of symmetry in orientation illusions , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.
[13] P D Zelazo,et al. The emergence of consciousness. , 1998, Advances in neurology.
[14] A. Diamond,et al. The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task. , 1989, Developmental psychobiology.
[15] C. Blakemore,et al. Lateral Inhibition between Orientation Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1970, Nature.
[16] Linda B. Smith,et al. Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: the influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of two-year-olds. , 2001, Child development.
[17] Jack L. Vevea,et al. Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[18] P. V. Geert,et al. A dynamic systems model of basic developmental mechanisms: Piaget, Vygotsky, and beyond , 1998 .
[19] C. Ghez,et al. Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] A. Diamond. Developmental Time Course in Human Infants and Infant Monkeys, and the Neural Bases of, Inhibitory Control in Reaching a , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[21] J. Huttenlocher,et al. The development of hierarchical representation of two-dimensional space. , 1996, Child development.
[22] E. Thelen,et al. The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[23] H. Schaffer,et al. Visual-manipulative response strategies in infant operant conditioning with spatially displaced feedback. , 1973, British journal of psychology.
[24] James L. McClelland,et al. Rethinking infant knowledge: toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. , 1997, Psychological review.
[25] Adele Diamond,et al. AB with multiple wells. I: Why are multiple wells sometimes easier than two wells?. II: memory or memory + inhibition? , 1994 .
[26] F. J. Langdon,et al. The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .
[27] J. Gordon,et al. Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] J. Reznick,et al. Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. , 1998, Developmental psychology.
[29] J. Reznick,et al. Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. , 1998, Developmental psychology.
[30] G. Schöner,et al. The Dynamic Neural Field Theory of Motor Programming: Arm and Eye Movements , 1997 .
[31] C. T. Ramey,et al. Delayed reinforcement and vocalization rates of infants. , 1971, Child development.
[32] Nora S. Newcombe,et al. THE DEVELOPMENT OF SPATIAL LOCATION CODING: PLACE LEARNING AND DEAD RECKONING IN THE SECOND AND THIRD YEARS , 1998 .
[33] L. Brody. Visual short-term cued recall memory in infancy. , 1981, Child development.
[34] D. Rosenbaum. Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[35] J. Watson. Memory and "contingency analysis" in infant learning. , 1967 .
[36] Colin Blakemore,et al. Interactions between orientations in human vision , 1973, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] L. J. Friedman. Child Development , 1971, Nature.
[38] Linda B. Smith,et al. Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. , 1999, Psychological review.
[39] S. Amari. Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields , 1977, Biological Cybernetics.
[40] L. Hedges,et al. Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. , 1991, Psychological review.
[41] H. Bouma,et al. Induced changes in the perceived orientation of line segments. , 1970, Vision research.
[42] Julie Messier,et al. Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[43] Peter Wenderoth,et al. Theangular function of arod-and-frame illusion , 1971 .
[44] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Making Space: The Development of Spatial Representation and Reasoning , 2000 .