It's All About You: An ERP Study of Emotion and Self-Relevance in Discourse

Accurately communicating self-relevant and emotional information is a vital function of language, but we have little idea about how these factors impact normal discourse comprehension. In an event-related potential (ERP) study, we fully crossed self-relevance and emotion in a discourse context. Two-sentence social vignettes were presented either in the third or the second person (previous work has shown that this influences the perspective from which mental models are built). ERPs were time-locked to a critical word toward the end of the second sentence which was pleasant, neutral, or unpleasant (e.g., A man knocks on Sandra's/your hotel room door. She/You see(s) that he has agift/tray/gunin his hand.). We saw modulation of early components (P1, N1, and P2) by self-relevance, suggesting that a self-relevant context can lead to top-down attentional effects during early stages of visual processing. Unpleasant words evoked a larger late positivity than pleasant words, which evoked a larger positivity than neutral words, indicating that, regardless of self-relevance, emotional words are assessed as motivationally significant, triggering additional or deeper processing at post-lexical stages. Finally, self-relevance and emotion interacted on the late positivity: a larger late positivity was evoked by neutral words in self-relevant, but not in non-self-relevant, contexts. This may reflect prolonged attempts to disambiguate the emotional valence of ambiguous stimuli that are relevant to the self. More broadly, our findings suggest that the assessment of emotion and self-relevance are not independent, but rather that they interactively influence one another during word-by-word language comprehension.

[1]  Hong Li,et al.  The Temporal Features of Self-referential Processing Evoked by Chinese Handwriting , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  N. Moray Attention in Dichotic Listening: Affective Cues and the Influence of Instructions , 1959 .

[3]  L. Abramson,et al.  Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  Hideki Ohira,et al.  An ERP study on self-relevant object recognition , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[5]  Michael Inzlicht,et al.  The Devil You Know , 2008, Psychological science.

[6]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Evaluation: fMRI Correlates of Valence, Emotional Intensity, and Control in the Processing of Attitudes , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Jonas K. Olofsson,et al.  Affective picture processing: An integrative review of ERP findings , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[8]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Both sides get the point: Hemispheric sensitivities to sentential constraint , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[10]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  James W. Tanaka,et al.  Activation of Preexisting and Acquired Face Representations: The N250 Event-related Potential as an Index of Face Familiarity , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  E. Fox,et al.  Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  John J. Foxe,et al.  My face or yours? Event-related potential correlates of self-face processing , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  Dan Foti,et al.  Tell me about it: neural activity elicited by emotional pictures and preceding descriptions. , 2009, Emotion.

[15]  Yue-Jia Luo,et al.  Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: An ERP study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[16]  A. Nowicka,et al.  Allocation of attention to self-name and self-face: An ERP study , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[17]  J. Russell,et al.  The neurophysiological bases of emotion: An fMRI study of the affective circumplex using emotion‐denoting words , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[18]  S. Geisser,et al.  On methods in the analysis of profile data , 1959 .

[19]  Gina R. Kuperberg,et al.  Neurophysiological Correlates of Comprehending Emotional Meaning in Context , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Curt Burgess,et al.  Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence , 1996 .

[21]  Meredith A. Shafto,et al.  Relations between emotion, memory, and attention: Evidence from taboo Stroop, lexical decision, and immediate memory tasks , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[22]  I. Jentzsch,et al.  Seeing yourself in a positive light: Brain correlates of the self-positivity bias , 2007, Brain Research.

[23]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence of Implicit and Explicit Categorization Processes , 2000 .

[24]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[25]  Simon Rigoulot,et al.  Arousal and valence effects on event-related P3a and P3b during emotional categorization. , 2006, International Journal of Psychophysiology.

[26]  T. Ethofer,et al.  His or mine? The time course of self–other discrimination in emotion processing , 2011, Social neuroscience.

[27]  H Pratt,et al.  P300 in response to the subject's own name. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  Tomohiro Takezawa,et al.  Activation of Self-Knowledge Reduces Conflict During Occupational Choice: An ERP Study , 2009, The International journal of neuroscience.

[29]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  R. Compton The interface between emotion and attention: a review of evidence from psychology and neuroscience. , 2003, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[31]  Dinkar Sharma,et al.  Intrusive Cognitions: An Investigation of the Emotional Stroop Task , 1995 .

[32]  Joachim I. Krueger,et al.  The Self in Social Judgment , 2013 .

[33]  Annmarie MacNamara,et al.  ERPs and the Study of Emotion , 2011 .

[34]  A. Ohman,et al.  The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  M. Bradley,et al.  Brain potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[36]  J. Kissler,et al.  Emotion and attention in visual word processing—An ERP study , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[37]  J. Bargh Attention and Automaticity in the Processing of Self-Relevant Information , 1982 .

[38]  Wei Li,et al.  Perspective taking modulates event-related potentials to perceived pain , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[39]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[40]  G. Hajcak,et al.  Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: evidence from temporal-spatial PCA. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[41]  G. Hajcak,et al.  Event-Related Potentials, Emotion, and Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review , 2010, Developmental neuropsychology.

[42]  Yue Ge,et al.  Anxiety and outcome evaluation: The good, the bad and the ambiguous , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[43]  Felicia Pratto,et al.  Individual construct accessibility and perceptual selection , 1986 .

[44]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .

[45]  P. Pauli,et al.  Self-reference modulates the processing of emotional stimuli in the absence of explicit self-referential appraisal instructions. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[46]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  Modulation of the brain activity in outcome evaluation by interpersonal relationship: An ERP study , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  Tad T. Brunyé,et al.  When You and I Share Perspectives , 2009, Psychological science.

[49]  H. Gray,et al.  P300 as an index of attention to self-relevant stimuli , 2004 .

[50]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion , 2001 .

[51]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  The English Lexicon Project , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[52]  Kevin Allan,et al.  Self-Reference During Explicit Memory Retrieval , 2007, Psychological science.

[53]  Jiang Qiu,et al.  The neural basis of self-evaluation processing in social judgment , 2010, Neuroreport.

[54]  A. Anderson Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[55]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Neural mechanisms involved in the detection of our first name: a combined ERPs and PET study , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: When Predictions Fail: The Dilemma of Unrealistic Optimism , 2002 .

[58]  Suzanne Mannes,et al.  Global Semantic Expectancy and Language Comprehension , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  O. John,et al.  Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[60]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affective words. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  Anna Weinberg,et al.  Beyond good and evil: the time-course of neural activity elicited by specific picture content. , 2010, Emotion.

[62]  F. Martin,et al.  Affective picture processing and motivational relevance: arousal and valence effects on ERPs in an oddball task. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[63]  Margaret Bradley,et al.  Event-related potential studies of language and emotion: words, phrases, and task effects. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[64]  Gina R Kuperberg,et al.  Dysfunction of a cortical midline network during emotional appraisals in schizophrenia. , 2011, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[65]  C. H. Hansen,et al.  Finding the face in the crowd: an anger superiority effect. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[66]  Shihui Han,et al.  Cultural difference in neural mechanisms of self-recognition , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[67]  Marta Kutas,et al.  Episodic Action Memory for Real Objects: An ERP Investigation With Perform, Watch, and Imagine Action Encoding Tasks Versus a Non-Action Encoding Task , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  Jan W. Van Strien,et al.  Processing of pleasant information can be as fast and strong as unpleasant information: implications for the negativity bias , 2008 .

[69]  Markus Junghöfer,et al.  Selective Visual Attention to Emotion , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[71]  Ramin Assadollahi,et al.  Emotional and semantic networks in visual word processing: insights from ERP studies. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[72]  Charles D. Yingling,et al.  Auditory P3 Responses to Name Stimuli , 1997, Brain and Language.

[73]  L. Watson Is it through emotion that we know ourselves? : A psychophysiological investigation into self-reference and emotional valence , 2008 .

[74]  P. Shaver,et al.  Cognitive consequences of self-awareness , 1976 .

[75]  Peter J. Lang,et al.  Gaze Patterns When Looking at Emotional Pictures: Motivationally Biased Attention , 2004 .

[76]  T. Onitsuka,et al.  P300 in response to the subject's own face , 1998, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[77]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Attentional shifts to emotionally charged cues: Behavioural and ERP data. , 1995 .

[78]  H. M. Müller,et al.  What's in a name? Electrophysiological differences between spoken nouns, proper names and one's own name , 1996, Neuroreport.

[79]  Olesya Govorun,et al.  Better-than-Average Effect , 2020, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.

[80]  J. Hyönä,et al.  Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. , 2006, Emotion.

[81]  Avgusta Y Shestyuk,et al.  Automatic and strategic representation of the self in major depression: trait and state abnormalities. , 2010, The American journal of psychiatry.