Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech

Abstract Comparisons between infant-directed and adult-directed speech were conducted to determine whether word-final syllables are highlighted in infant-directed speech. Samples of adult-directed and infant-directed speech were collected from 8 mothers of 6-month-old and 8 mothers of 9- month-old infants. Mothers were asked to label seven objects both to an experimenter and to their infant. Duration, pitch, and amplitude were measured for whole words and for each of the target word syllables. As in prior research, the infant-directed targets were higher pitched and longer than adult-directed targets. The results also extend beyond previous results in showing that lengthening of final syllables in infant-directed speech is particularly exaggerated. Results of analyses comparing word-final versus nonfinal unstressed syllables in utterance-medial position in infant-directed speech showed that lengthening of unstressed word-final syllables occurs even in utterance-internal positions. These results could suggest a mechanism for proposals that word-final syllables are perceptually salient to young children.

[1]  J. Morgan A Rhythmic Bias in Preverbal Speech Segmentation , 1996 .

[2]  S. Hura,et al.  The role of stress and articulatory difficulty in children's early productions , 1996 .

[3]  Performance Constraints in Early Language: The Case of Subjectless Sentences. , 1990 .

[4]  James L. Morgan,et al.  Signal to syntax : bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition , 1996 .

[5]  F Wijnen,et al.  The (non)realization of unstressed elements in children's utterances: evidence for a rhythmic constraint , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[6]  C. Fowler,et al.  Talkers' signaling of new and old. words in speech and listeners' perception and use of the distinction , 1987 .

[7]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .

[8]  A. Fernald,et al.  A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[9]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Perception of a phonetic contrast in multisyllabic utterances by 2-month-old infants , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  R. Gelman,et al.  The development of communication skills: modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. , 1973, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[11]  A. Fernald,et al.  Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults , 1991 .

[12]  P. Jensen,et al.  Stress and word position as determinants of imitation in first-language learners. , 1970, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[13]  J. Hayes Cognition and the development of language , 1970 .

[14]  Sarah Hawkins,et al.  Chapter 12 – RETRACTED: PHONOLOGICAL RHYTHM: DEFINITION AND DEVELOPMENT , 1980 .

[15]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. , 1993, Child development.

[16]  L. Nakatani,et al.  Hearing "words" without words: prosodic cues for word perception. , 1978, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  D. J. Foss,et al.  On the Role of Sentence Stress in Sentence Processing , 1977, Language and speech.

[18]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  The Role of Stress and Position in Determining First Words , 1992 .

[19]  A. Fernald,et al.  Expanded Intonation Contours in Mothers' Speech to Newborns. , 1984 .

[20]  James L. Morgan,et al.  From Simple Input to Complex Grammar , 1986 .

[21]  Frederick J. Newmeyer Language : psychological and biological aspects , 1988 .

[22]  A. Woodward,et al.  Perception of acoustic correlates of major phrasal units by young infants , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  Karen E. Pollock,et al.  An acoustic analysis of young children’s productions of word stress , 1993 .

[24]  J. Gee,et al.  Prosodic structure and spoken word recognition , 1987, Cognition.

[25]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .

[26]  H. Klein,et al.  Learning to stress: a case study , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[27]  L. Gerken,et al.  A metrical template account of children's weak syllable omissions from multisyllabic words , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[28]  D. Slobin,et al.  Studies of child language development , 1973 .

[29]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. , 1988 .

[30]  W. Zonneveld Syllables and segments : Alan Bell and Joan B. Hooper (eds.), North-Holland Linguistic Series 40. Papers from the Symposium on Segment Organization and the Syllable, Boulder, Colorado, October 21-23, 1977. North_Holland Publ. Co., Amsterdam, 1978 , 1980 .

[31]  K. Nelson,et al.  Structure and strategy in learning to talk. , 1973 .

[32]  Eric Wanner,et al.  Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .

[33]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech , 1987 .

[34]  J. Neter,et al.  Applied linear statistical models : regression, analysis of variance, and experimental designs , 1974 .

[35]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  Young Children′s Representation of Prosodic Phonology: Evidence From English-Speakers′ Weak Syllable Productions , 1994 .

[36]  Terri Gullickson,et al.  Annals of Child Development , 1995 .

[37]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition , 1979 .

[38]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Prosody: Models and measurements , 1983 .

[39]  Lloyd H. Nakatani,et al.  Prosodic Aspects of American English Speech Rhythm , 1981 .

[40]  Catharine H. Echols,et al.  The perception of rhythmic units in speech by infants and adults. , 1997 .

[41]  F. Newmeyer Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey , 1989 .

[42]  Bruce Hayes,et al.  Iambic and Trochaic Rhythm in Stress Rules , 1985 .

[43]  D. Slobin Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar , 1973 .

[44]  D. Slobin Crosslinguistic Evidence for the Language-making Capacity , 1985 .

[45]  P. Dale,et al.  Discrimination of linguistic stress in early infancy. , 1977, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[46]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Why Nouns Are Learned before Verbs: Linguistic Relativity Versus Natural Partitioning. Technical Report No. 257. , 1982 .

[47]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults , 1991 .

[48]  D. Klatt Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: acoustic and perceptual evidence. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[49]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Clauses are perceptual units for young infants , 1987, Cognition.

[50]  Eduardo D. Faingold,et al.  On the acquisition of prosodic structure , 1997 .

[51]  J. L. Myers Fundamentals of Experimental Design , 1972 .

[52]  J R Saffran,et al.  Emerging integration of sequential and suprasegmental information in preverbal speech segmentation. , 1995, Child development.

[53]  Nan Bernstein Ratner,et al.  Durational cues which mark clause boundaries in mother–child speech , 1986 .

[54]  A. Cutler,et al.  Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: Evidence from juncture misperception , 1992 .

[55]  D. Slobin The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition , 1987 .

[56]  Gillian R Brown,et al.  Prosodic Structure and the Given/New Distinction , 1983 .

[57]  Anne Cutler,et al.  The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access , 1988 .

[58]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey: Where learning begins: initial representations for language learning , 1988 .

[59]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A precursor of language acquisition in young infants , 1988, Cognition.

[60]  K. Demuth,et al.  The prosodic structure of early words , 1996 .

[61]  Ann M. Peters,et al.  The Units of Language Acquisition , 1983 .

[62]  H. Klein,et al.  Early perceptual strategies for the replication of consonants from polysyllabic lexical models. , 1981, Journal of speech and hearing research.