Anticipatory distractor suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual search
暂无分享,去创建一个
Jan Theeuwes | Eduard Ort | Joram van Driel | Benchi Wang | J. Theeuwes | Benchi Wang | J. V. Driel | E. Ort
[1] J. Theeuwes,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence of the Capture of Visual Attention , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[2] Steven J Luck,et al. Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[3] C. Olivers,et al. Posterior α EEG Dynamics Dissociate Current from Future Goals in Working Memory-Guided Visual Search , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[4] C. Koch,et al. Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[5] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional , 2018, Journal of cognition.
[6] Leonardo Chelazzi,et al. Altering spatial priority maps via statistical learning of target selection and distractor filtering , 2017, Cortex.
[7] Jürgen Kayser,et al. Issues and considerations for using the scalp surface Laplacian in EEG/ERP research: A tutorial review. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[8] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Statistical Regularities Modulate Attentional Capture , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] G. V. Simpson,et al. Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[10] W. Klimesch. Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[11] Edward K Vogel,et al. Neural Evidence for the Contribution of Active Suppression During Working Memory Filtering , 2019, Cerebral cortex.
[12] Steven J Luck,et al. Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[13] O. Jensen,et al. Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[14] Joy J. Geng,et al. Evidence for Second-Order Singleton Suppression Based on Probabilistic Expectations , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] Jeffrey D. Schall,et al. Prefrontal Control of Visual Distraction , 2017, Current Biology.
[16] Anna Schubö,et al. You see what you have learned. Evidence for an interrelation of associative learning and visual selective attention. , 2015, Psychophysiology.
[17] S J Luck,et al. Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[18] Michael X Cohen,et al. Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .
[19] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture independent of search strategy , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[20] J. Theeuwes. Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[21] W. Klimesch. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.
[22] M. Chun,et al. Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation , 1999 .
[23] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[24] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Spatial suppression due to statistical regularities is driven by distractor suppression not by target activation , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[25] Jeff Miller,et al. Measurement of ERP latency differences: a comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods. , 2008, Psychophysiology.
[26] John J. Foxe,et al. Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[27] M. Eimer. The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[28] Marta Kutas,et al. Identifying reliable independent components via split-half comparisons , 2009, NeuroImage.
[29] Agnieszka Wykowska,et al. Context heterogeneity has a sustained impact on attention deployment: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2013, Psychophysiology.
[30] M. Chun,et al. Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[31] F. Perrin,et al. Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[32] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Goal-driven, stimulus-driven, and history-driven selection. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.
[33] J. Theeuwes. Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[34] Clayton Hickey,et al. A temporal dependency account of attentional inhibition in oculomotor control , 2017, NeuroImage.
[35] John M. Gaspar,et al. Suppression of Salient Objects Prevents Distraction in Visual Search , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] Steven J. Luck,et al. ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[37] Roberto Dell'Acqua,et al. Contralateral cortical organisation of information in visual short-term memory: Evidence from lateralized brain activity during retrieval , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[38] Vincent Di Lollo,et al. Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[39] Jan Theeuwes,et al. How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top-down suppression , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[40] H. Egeth,et al. Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[41] M. Chun,et al. Selective attention modulates implicit learning , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[42] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional; A Reply to Commentaries , 2018, Journal of cognition.
[43] Heleen A. Slagter,et al. Learning What Is Irrelevant or Relevant: Expectations Facilitate Distractor Inhibition and Target Facilitation through Distinct Neural Mechanisms , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[44] Robert Oostenveld,et al. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..
[45] J. Theeuwes,et al. Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[46] R. Oostenveld,et al. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[47] David J. Prime,et al. Inability to suppress salient distractors predicts low visual working memory capacity , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[48] S. Yantis,et al. Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.
[49] Heleen A. Slagter,et al. Learning What Is Irrelevant or Relevant: Expectations Facilitate Distractor Inhibition and Target Facilitation through Distinct Neural Mechanisms , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[50] Eren Gunseli,et al. Local and interregional alpha EEG dynamics dissociate between memory for search and memory for recognition , 2017, NeuroImage.
[51] W. Klimesch. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[52] Katherine M. Armstrong,et al. Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2003, Nature.
[53] Steven J. Luck,et al. The Role of Inhibition in Avoiding Distraction by Salient Stimuli , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[54] Ali Jannati,et al. Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[56] J. Theeuwes. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[57] W. Ziegler. The Oxford Handbook Of Event Related Potential Components , 2016 .
[58] Steven J. Luck,et al. ERP Components and Selective Attention , 2011 .
[59] Steven J. Luck,et al. Combined Electrophysiological and Behavioral Evidence for the Suppression of Salient Distractors , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[60] Dirk Kerzel,et al. The distractor positivity (Pd) signals lowering of attentional priority: evidence from event-related potentials and individual differences. , 2014, Psychophysiology.
[61] Howard E Egeth,et al. The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.