GreekLex: A lexical database of Modern Greek

To conduct experimental investigations into the orthographic processing of Modern Greek, information is needed about the lexical properties known to influence visual word recognition. In this article we introduce GreekLex, a lexical database for Modern Greek, which presents collectively for the first time a series of orthographic measures that can be used for psycholinguistic research. GreekLex consists of 35,304 Modern Greek words ranging in length from 1 to 22 letters, and for each word includes the following statistical information: word length, word-form frequency, lemma frequency, neighborhood density and frequency, transposition neighbors, and addition and deletion neighbors. Furthermore, type and token frequency measures of single letters and bigrams derived from the database are also available. The complete database can be accessed and downloaded freely from www.psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/GreekLex.

[1]  S. Andrews The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts , 1997 .

[2]  M. S. Mayzner,et al.  Tables of single-letter and digram frequency counts for various word-length and letter-position combinations. , 1965 .

[3]  Sally Andrews,et al.  Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Lexical similarity or orthographic redundancy? , 1992 .

[4]  Robert Schreuder,et al.  How Complex Simplex Words can be , 1997 .

[5]  Robert L. Solso,et al.  Positional frequency and versatility of bigrams for two- through nine-letter English words , 1980 .

[6]  A. Pollatsek,et al.  The effects of neighborhood frequency in reading and lexical decision. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[8]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition , 1984 .

[9]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[10]  J. Grainger,et al.  An Electrophysiological Study of the Effects of Orthographic Neighborhood Size on Printed Word Perception , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Manuel Perea,et al.  The effects of orthographic neighborhood in reading and laboratory word identification tasks: A review , 2000 .

[12]  J. Grainger,et al.  Effects of orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition: cross-task comparisons. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  P. Mousty,et al.  Brulex: une base de donne 'es lexicales informatise 'e pour le franc?ais e 'crit et parle , 1990 .

[14]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  S. M. Chambers Letter and order information in lexical access , 1979 .

[16]  S. Andrews Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability , 1996 .

[17]  J Grainger,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.

[18]  Prisca Stenneken,et al.  Sublexical frequency measures for orthographic and phonological units in German , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[19]  S. Lupker,et al.  Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions ☆ , 2004 .

[20]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[21]  M. Taft,et al.  Lack of Phonological Mediation in a Semantic Categorization Task , 1998 .

[22]  Laurie Beth Feldman,et al.  Decomposing words into their constituent morphemes: evidence from English and Hebrew. , 1995 .

[23]  Max Coltheart,et al.  The MRC Psycholinguistic Database , 1981 .

[24]  George Carayannis,et al.  Basic Quantitative Characteristics of the Modern Greek Language Using the Hellenic National Corpus , 2005, J. Quant. Linguistics.

[25]  Manuel Carreiras,et al.  E-Hitz: A word frequency list and a program for deriving psycholinguistic statistics in an agglutinative language (Basque) , 2006, Behavior research methods.

[26]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Effects of word length and frequency on the human event-related potential , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[27]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Transposed-letter effects: Consonants, vowels and letter frequency , 2008 .

[28]  Manuel Perea,et al.  The effects of length and transposed-letter similarity in lexical decision: evidence with beginning, intermediate, and adult readers. , 2008, British journal of psychology.

[29]  J. Bowers,et al.  Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a hat in that? , 2005 .

[30]  C. Davis,et al.  More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  David A. Balota,et al.  Visual Word Recognition: The Journey from Features to Meaning (A Travel Update) , 2006 .

[32]  Kenneth I. Forster,et al.  No enemies in the neighborhood: absence of inhibitory neighborhood effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths , 2000, Psychological research.

[34]  Robert D Vincent,et al.  Anagram software for cognitive research that enables specification of psycholinguistic variables , 2006, Behavior research methods.

[35]  C. Davis N-Watch: A program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[36]  C. Davis,et al.  BuscaPalabras: A program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[37]  K. Rastle,et al.  The processing of singular and plural nouns in French and English , 2004 .

[38]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[39]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Transposed-Letter Confusability Effects in Masked Form Priming , 2003 .

[40]  S. Andrews Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Activation or search? , 1989 .

[41]  Cristina Burani,et al.  Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[42]  George Carayannis,et al.  Word Length, Word Frequencies and Zipf’s Law in the Greek Language , 2001, J. Quant. Linguistics.

[43]  Derek Besner,et al.  Phonological Encoding in the Lexical Decision Task , 1979 .

[44]  Heike Martensen,et al.  Who is dominating the Dutch neighbourhood? On the role of subsyllabic units in Dutch nonword reading , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[45]  S. Lupker,et al.  Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[46]  Philip T Quinlan,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[47]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English Lexicon Project , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Lexique 2 : A new French lexical database , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[49]  J. Grainger Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming. , 1990 .

[50]  J. Bowers Masked priming: State of the Art , 2003 .

[51]  Yasushi Hino,et al.  Neighborhood Size and Neighborhood Frequency Effects in Word Recognition , 1995 .

[52]  Chris Westbury,et al.  The Probability of the Least Likely Non-Length-Controlled Bigram Affects Lexical Decision Reaction Times , 2002, Brain and Language.

[53]  Michael N Jones,et al.  Case-sensitive letter and bigram frequency counts from large-scale English corpora , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[54]  A. Pollatsek,et al.  Reading Finnish compound words: eye fixations are affected by component morphemes. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[56]  R. Baayen,et al.  Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model , 1997 .

[57]  J. Grainger,et al.  Orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual word recognition , 1998 .

[58]  Brian D. Joseph,et al.  The Modern Greek Language: A Descriptive Analysis of Standard Modern Greek , 1985 .