Selective impairment of knowledge underlying un- prefixation: further evidence for the autonomy of grammatical semantics

This paper focuses on grammatical semantics, which is a level of linguistic representation that lies at the interface between syntactic categories and conceptual categories. We report a series of neuropsychological experiments that investigated the semantic constraints on the English verbal un- prefixation construction. In order for a verb to occur in this construction, it must designate an event which involves the creation of a constricted, reversible spatial configuration—e.g. wrap–unwrap, buckle–unbuckle, and clog–unclog, but not float–*unfloat, boil–*unboil, or fluff–*unfluff. Other features of verb meaning are completely invisible to the construction—e.g. the idiosyncratic features that distinguish between wrap, buckle, and clog. Of six brain-damaged patients who were studied, two exhibited the following dissociation. On the one hand, they performed well on a verb–picture matching test that required them to discriminate between subtle features of verb meaning that are not relevant to un- prefixation. On the other hand, they failed a grammaticality judgment test that required them to determine whether the very same verbs satisfy the narrow semantic criteria for the construction. A final control test demonstrated that their errors on the judgment test were not due to problems with certain task demands, but were instead most likely due to an impairment that selectively affected their knowledge or processing of grammatical semantics.

[1]  Cynthia Fisher,et al.  On the semantic content of subcategorization frames , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  Arnold M. Zwicky,et al.  Dealing Out Meaning: Fundamentals of Syntactic Constructions , 1994 .

[3]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Words in the brain's language , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[4]  Bernard Comrie,et al.  Approaches to the Typology of Word Classes , 2000 .

[5]  Adele E. Goldberg,et al.  The contribution of argument structure constructions to sentence meaning , 2000 .

[6]  Harold Goodglass,et al.  Anomia : neuroanatomical and cognitive correlates , 1997 .

[7]  David Kemmerer,et al.  Selective impairment of knowledge underlying prenominal adjective order: evidence for the autonomy of grammatical semantics , 2000, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[8]  M. Tomasello Do young children have adult syntactic competence? , 2000, Cognition.

[9]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Words and rules , 1998 .

[10]  D. Kemmerer Why can you hit someone on the arm but not break someone on the arm?—a neuropsychological investigation of the English body-part possessor ascension construction , 2003, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[11]  H. Marchand The categories and types of present-day English word-formation : a synchronic-diachronic approach , 1960 .

[12]  R. J. Frank,et al.  Three-dimensional in vivo mapping of brain lesions in humans. , 1992, Archives of neurology.

[13]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .

[14]  Shigeyasu Sawada,et al.  ON THE VERB-FORMING PREFIX UN - , 1995 .

[15]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  On the Neurology of Naming , 1997 .

[16]  Anna Wierzbicka The semantics of grammar , 1988 .

[17]  Anthony R. Davis,et al.  Linking as constraints on word classes in a hierarchical lexicon , 2000 .

[18]  Andrew J. Saykin,et al.  Neuropsychological Assessment of Neuropsychiatric Disorders , 1988 .

[19]  Laura A. Michaelis,et al.  TOWARD A CONSTRUCTION-BASED THEORY OF LANGUAGE FUNCTION : THE CASE OF NOMINAL EXTRAPOSITION , 1996 .

[20]  W. Bruce Croft Radical Construction Grammar , 2001 .

[21]  W. Nelson Francis,et al.  FREQUENCY ANALYSIS OF ENGLISH USAGE: LEXICON AND GRAMMAR , 1983 .

[22]  Jane Marshall,et al.  Calling a salad a federation: An investigation of semantic jargon. Part 1—nouns , 1996, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[23]  D. Howard,et al.  Why Is a Verb Like an Inanimate Object? Grammatical Category and Semantic Category Deficits , 2000, Brain and Language.

[24]  M. Tomasello The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  E. Clark,et al.  Reference states and reversals: undoing actions with verbs , 1995, Journal of Child Language.

[26]  S. Pinker How could a child use verb syntax to learn verb semantics , 1994 .

[27]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Grammatical class in lexical production and morhpological processing: Evidence from a case of fluent aphasia , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[28]  E. Saffran,et al.  Sentence processing in the face of semantic loss: a case study. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  Edna Andrews A SYNCHRONIC SEMANTIC ANALYSIS OF DE- AND UN- IN AMERICAN ENGLISH , 1986 .

[30]  B. Levin Unaccusativity: At the Syntax-Lexical Semantics Interface , 1994 .

[31]  Wolf-Peter Funk On the semantic and morphological status of reversative verbs in English and German , 1991 .

[32]  Randy J. LaPolla,et al.  Syntax: Structure, Meaning, and Function , 1999 .

[33]  David R. Dowty,et al.  Word Meaning and Montague Grammar , 1979 .

[34]  B. L. Whorf Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf , 1956 .

[35]  S. J. Keyser,et al.  The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger , 1993 .

[36]  A. Goldberg Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure , 1995 .

[37]  Eric Wanner,et al.  Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .

[38]  Michael Gasser,et al.  The Acquisition of the Lexicon , 1997 .

[39]  J. Hay Lexical frequency in morphology: Is everything relative? , 2001 .

[40]  Beth Levin,et al.  English Verb Classes and Alternations: A Preliminary Investigation , 1993 .

[41]  Macklin Thomas Interchangeable Pairs in Un-, In-, En-, etc. , 1983 .

[42]  C. Fillmore,et al.  Regularity and Idiomaticity in Grammatical Constructions: The Case of Let Alone , 1988 .

[43]  S. Pinker Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure , 1989 .

[44]  Adele E. Goldberg,et al.  Conceptual structure, discourse and language , 1996 .

[45]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Patterns of dissociation in the processing of verb meanings in brain-damaged subjects , 2001 .

[46]  Paul Kay,et al.  Words and the grammar of context , 1997, CSLI lecture notes series.

[47]  Helen Vendler,et al.  Poems, Poets, Poetry: An Introduction and Anthology , 2002 .

[48]  D. Caplan,et al.  Language: Structure, Processing, and Disorders , 1994 .

[49]  J. Hay Causes and Consequences of Word Structure , 2003 .

[50]  A. Colen On the Distribution of un-, de-and dis-in English Verbs Expressing Reversativity and Related Concepts , 1980 .

[51]  David Kemmerer,et al.  Grammatically relevant and grammatically irrelevant features of verb meaning can be independently impaired , 2000 .

[52]  Marjolyn Verspoor,et al.  Lexical and syntactical constructions and the construction of meaning : proceedings of the bi-annual ICLA meeting in Albuquerque, July 1995 , 1997 .

[53]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. , 1994 .

[54]  John B. Carroll,et al.  The American Heritage Word Frequency Book , 1971 .

[55]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. , 1987 .

[56]  Melissa Bowerman,et al.  Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development , 1982 .

[57]  Miriam Butt,et al.  The Projection of Arguments: Lexical and Compositional Factors , 2004 .

[58]  H. Damasio Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images , 1995 .

[59]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Rules, representations, and the English past tense , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Ray Jackendoff TWISTIN' THE NIGHT AWAY , 1997 .

[61]  W. Bruce Croft Parts of speech as language universals and as language-particular categories , 2000 .

[62]  Ken Hale,et al.  On Argument Structure and the Lexical Expression of Syntactic Relations , 1993 .

[63]  M. Silveri,et al.  Noun-verb dissociations in brain-damaged patients: Further evidence , 1997 .

[64]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The organization of semantic memory. , 2001 .